Skip to main content
Login | Suomeksi | På svenska | In English

Browsing by master's degree program "Psykologian maisteriohjelma"

Sort by: Order: Results:

  • Frilander, Nelli (2024)
    Objective: Anxiety disorders are among the most common groups of mental disorders, and one of the leading causes of disease burden globally. Anxiety disorders are associated with many general medical conditions (GMCs), but little studies of the wide-ranging associations over multiple GMC categories have been conducted. This study aims to examine whether people with an anxiety disorder have more lifetime GMCs compared to people without anxiety disorders, and whether they have a higher risk of developing subsequent GMCs, across multiple GMC categories. Methods: The participants were a part of the FinnGen study sample, totaling 519 841 individuals. Lifetime comorbidity analyses were conducted with an anxiety disorder group (N = 63 695) consisted of all individuals who had any ICD-10 phobic anxiety disorder or other anxiety disorder diagnosis, or corresponding ICD-9 or ICD-8 diagnosis. A sex and age (±2 years) matched control group (N = 127 390) of individuals who had no phobic anxiety disorder or other anxiety disorder diagnoses was created. Proportions of GMCs from 11 different categories (allergy, gastrointestinal, urogenital, hematological, neurological, dermatological, musculoskeletal, endocrine, pulmonary, oncological, and cardiovascular) in anxiety disorder group and matched control group were compared with each other with χ2-test. Next, Cox regression was used to calculate whether having an anxiety disorder increases the risk of developing a subsequent GMC. The whole FinnGen sample was used as a basis for the Cox regression analyses, and for each analysis, the individuals who had a diagnosis in the corresponding GMC category before the start of follow-up period (1.1.2000–31.12.2020) were removed from the analysis, leading to samples whose sizes varied between 451 523 and 508 804. Four Cox regression models were created for each GMC category: Model A, which was adjusted for sex, age, and calendar time, Model B that was further adjusted for other GMC comorbidities, and time-lagged versions of Models A and B. Results: Individuals with anxiety disorders had higher lifetime prevalence rates of all GMCs, except for cancers. Having a prevalent anxiety disorder diagnosis increased the risk of a subsequent GMC in all 11 GMC categories, most notably gastrointestinal and hematological conditions. The hazard ratios ranged between 1.21 for cancers and 2.59 for hematological conditions. Conclusions: There are wide-ranging associations between anxiety disorders and GMCs. For most GMCs, individuals with an anxiety disorder had about two-fold risk to develop a subsequent GMC. The associations were especially strong for gastrointestinal and hematological conditions, and clearly the weakest for cancers. This study provides evidence of temporal associations across a range of GMCs, also including ones that have hardly been studied previously, such as hematological conditions and allergies. Further study with separate anxiety disorders and GMCs, as well as genetic connections, is still needed.
  • Vartiainen, Erika (2020)
    Aims In previous research, the focus has been on how single factors (e.g. change of school, class or school size) are connected to motivation. No study has used a comprehensive set of aspects of school stability and investigated its association with school motivation. Further, no study has examined whether temperament modifies the connection between the stability of school environment and motivation. This study investigated 1) whether the instability of school environment is associated with students’ motivation and 2) whether this association is modified by temperament. Methods The sample consisted of 3369 Finnish ninth graders. Students’ school motivation and temperament traits (task orientation, reactivity and flexibility) were measured by self-report and teachers’ evaluation. A wide array of factors within school environment were assessed (by asking students and teachers), and three instability scales were formed: instability of physical space, social relationships, and general school structure. Results When adding all the instability scales to the model as predictors simultaneously, high instability in social relations and high instability of physical space were associated with lower student-rated and teacher-rated motivation. Instability of general school structure did not have significant main effect on student-rated or teacher-rated motivation. When adding all the two-way interactions between temperament traits and instability scales to the model as predictors simultaneously, temperament was found to have modifying effects on motivation: high reactivity modified both, the connection of instability of physical spaces and instability of social relationships on student-rated motivation in students. High flexibility modified the connection of instability of general school structure on student-rated motivation. High task orientation modified the connection of instability of physical spaces on teacher-rated motivation. In all the models, covariates included also students’ age, gender, and parental education. Conclusions Various dimensions of instability of school environment are differentially connected with students’ motivation. Temperament traits appear to modify these connections, indicating that different temperaments become differently motivated in instable school environments.
  • Hasanen, Allan (2020)
    Tavoitteet. Intuitiivinen ja analyyttinen ajattelutyyli vaikuttavat tiedon käsittelyn syvyyteen ja omaan päättelyyn luottamiseen. Episteeminen kypsyys puolestaan ilmenee kehittyneenä tapana hahmottaa tiedon objektiivisen ja subjektiivisen dimension eroavaisuuksia, sekä ymmärryksenä tiedon monimutkaisuudesta ja varmuudesta. Tässä pro gradu -tutkielmassa tarkastellaan, voidaanko ajattelutyyleillä ennustaa vaihtelua episteemisessä kypsyydessä. Lisäksi tutkittiin, voidaanko avoimella ajattelutyylillä tai kyvyllä kognitiiviseen reflektioon ennustaa episteemistä kypsyyttä. Menetelmät. Otos koostui 460:sta 16-71- vuotiaasta suomalaisesta. Koehenkilöistä miehiä oli 19.3%, naisia 76.3% ja 4,3% ei valinnut kumpaakaan näistä vaihtoehdoista. Tutkimus toteutettiin internet-kyselynä, jonka koehenkilöt rekrytoitiin keskustelufoorumeilta ja opiskelijoiden sähköpostilistoilta. Koehenkilöt vastasivat kysymyksiin, joilla kartoitettiin heidän intuitiivista- analyyttistä- ja avointa ajattelutyyliä, kognitiivista reflektiota. Episteemistä kypsyyttä kartoitettiin tutkimalla episteemistä tasoa ja tiedon yksinkertaisuuteen/varmuuteen liittyviä tietokäsityksiä. Tulokset ja johtopäätökset. Intuitiivinen ajattelutyyli ennusti matalaa episteemistä tasoa ja korkeaa uskomusta tiedon yksinkertaisuudesta, mutta ei uskomuksia tiedon epävarmuudesta. Analyyttinen ajattelutyyli ennusti matalaa uskoa tiedon yksinkertaisuudesta, mutta ei korkeampaa episteemistä tasoa tai uskomuksia tiedon varmuudesta. Kriittinen reflektio puolestaan ennusti korkeampaa episteemistä tasoa ja uskomusta tiedon varmuuteen, sekä matalampaa uskomusta tiedon yksinkertaisuudesta. Tämän lisäksi matala avoin ajattelutyyli ainoastaan yhdessä korkean kognitiivisen reflektion kanssa ennustivat kokemusta tiedon varmuudesta. Rajoituksena tutkimuksessa oli opiskelijoiden yliedustuvuus aineistossa. Tutkimuksella pyrittiin selittämään vaihtelua ihmisten episteemisessä kypsyydessä, sekä tuotiin henkilökohtaisen epistemologian ja ajattelutyylien tutkimusaloja lähemmäs toisiaan.
  • Riikonen, Sanna-Maria (2023)
    Objectives. The five-factor model of personality is the most researched and widely accepted theory of the structure of personality. It is claimed to be a near universal even though studies of the model have overwhelmingly relied on samples from Western, educated, industrialized, rich, and democratic (WEIRD) populations and/or university or college students from non-WEIRD countries. Therefore, multiple scholars have called for studies that examine personality traits with other kinds of samples, but only relatively few studies like that have been conducted. The aims of this study were to examine whether the model is suitable to explain personality variation in elephant handlers, or mahouts, from Myanmar, and, if it is not applicable, how personality appears to manifest in the mahouts. Testing whether the model applies to this sample is highly valuable for assessing the model’s cultural variability and the claims about its near universality. Methods. The sample (n = 237) consisted of 138 mahouts and 99 of their peers, most of whom were fellow mahouts. They gave self- or observer ratings to an interviewer asking questions from the 44-item Big Five Inventory (BFI), which was translated into Burmese. Self-observer agreement and retest correlations were also calculated. The personality structure of the 138 mahouts was investigated with confirmatory and exploratory factor analysis. Results. The confirmatory factor analysis resulted in a covariance matrix of latent variables that was not positive-definite possibly because of the small sample size. The analysis method was then changed into exploratory factor analysis, and models with four, five, and six factors were com-pared in more detail. However, no clean factor structure emerged since the items measuring Open-ness to Experience, Conscientiousness, Extraversion, Agreeableness, and Neuroticism in the BFI did not, in general, load in a clear or sensible way in any of the factor solutions. Conclusions. These results could provide support for the hypothesis that mahout personality cannot be described with the five factors. Their personality variation may instead be organized along differently composed dimensions. This study could therefore be another example of the five-factor model of being more culture-specific than (near) universal.
  • Ståhl, Aada (2022)
    Objectives The human-pet attachment can substantially impact the life of the human and the pet. A few studies suggest that human personality and unwanted behaviour of dogs are related to pet attachment, but the relationship between pet personality and the attachment has not yet been studied. The aim of this study was to elucidate the links from human, dog (Canis familiaris), and cat (Felis silvestris catus) personalities and unwanted pet behaviour to the attachment. Methods The online survey data, from the OnePersonality project, captured responses of 2 724 pet owners (92,1% women) to the Short Five questionnaire, the Pet Attachment Questionnaire and the dog and cat personality and behaviour questionnaires. I utilised a series of linear and generalized linear mixed-effect models to examine the associations. Results Neuroticism was associated with attachment anxiety to dogs and cats. Dog owners scoring lower on extraversion and conscientiousness and owners of less human-social and more perseverant dogs were more anxiously attached. Cat’s human aggression, fearfulness and low human sociability associated with attachment anxiety and excessive grooming and lower human sociability with attachment avoidance. Less conscientious, extraverted, and agreeable dog owners and owners of more insecure, energetic, aggressive, less training focused, and less human social dogs were more avoidantly attached. Nine out of ten dog’s unwanted behaviour traits were related to avoidant attachment. Conclusions Both human and pet traits contribute to the owner-perceived attachment. Owner’s personality may have a bigger role in anxious attachment, while the dog’s individuality may be more related to attachment avoidance.
  • Simelius, Saana (2023)
    Leadership emergence and successful leadership are caused by a large group of individual characteristics and abilities, such as personality traits, gender, previous life experiences and cognitive abilities. According to previous studies, these individual qualities are associated with leadership outcomes via leadership motivation and leadership styles, which are also connected to each other. Cognitive ability is known to be connected to leadership outcomes; however, the mechanism of action has been left unclear by previous studies. In this study, the association of cognitive ability with leadership motivation and leadership styles were examined, while using the link between gender and leadership motivation and styles as a comparison point. Additionally, to establish the Model of Leadership Background Factors, connection between leadership motivation and leadership styles were examined. Data (n=603) was collected from Finnish personal assessment context where cognitive ability was measured with matrices testing, and leadership motivation and styles were assessed through self-rating questionnaires. Cognitive ability was not associated to either leadership motivation or leadership styles. Only small effects between coaching leadership style and lack of inspirational leadership were found, and they are likely to be explained by competitive people acquiring better than average results from the cognitive ability testing battery. On the other hand, gender made a difference in leadership motivation by women feeling less motivated to lead team’s actions and bigger picture of the business. Gender didn’t have a notable effect on leadership style apart from women scoring a bit higher on collaborative and responsibility sharing leadership. Finally, association between leadership motivation and leadership styles were found; higher leadership motivation tended to associate with strong, demanding leadership and lower motivation with collaborative and non-independent leadership. Knowledge of background factors in leadership outcomes enables appropriate assessment, selection and training of leaders, and this study already suggests that the Model of Leadership Background Factors might be useful in Finnish working life as well. This study supports leadership concepts as complex phenomena which need to be examined carefully also when it comes to challenging topics such as intelligence and gender.
  • Kettunen, Titta-Maria (2023)
    Objective The objective of this study is to examine the relationship between organizational justice and cognitive performance in working age population and whether this relationship differs between men and women. Organizational justice can be defined as perceived fairness of decisions and procedures at workplace. Organizational justice has been associated with cardiovascular risk and sleeping problems among other issues. Research indicates that organizational justice is associated also with cognitive performance, but this association has not been examined using population-based data earlier. Methods The sample for the study was derived from the Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns Study data. The sample used included all the participants from year 2012 who had participated to at least one cognitive subscale measurement and answered to at least four (50%) organizational justice question (n = 1283, women 57 %, average age 42,1 years). Organizational justice perceptions were measured using the short version of the organizational justice scale, which provided scores for total, distributive, procedural and interpersonal organizational justice. Cognitive performance was measured with four CANTAB tests. The results had been formed into five factors where the fifth factor represented the total cognitive performance. Linear regressions were used to examine the relationship both with and without covariates. Covariates used were age, sex, education level and average working hours per day. In addition, interactions of sex and organizational justice were examined. Results When the covariates were included in the analysis, both higher total scores of organizational justice and procedural justice were associated with shorter reaction times. Higher distributive justice was associated with better total cognitive performance and faster visual processing. The interactions of sex and total scores of organizational justice, procedural justice and interactional justice were each significant with total cognitive performance, executive function, and visual processing. The associations between organizational justice and cognitive performance were positive for men and negative for women. Conclusions Higher organizational justice seems to be associated with better cognitive performance especially regarding attention and psychomotor speed in working age population. The associations often differ significantly between men and women. Even though the associations found were negative for women, it can be concluded that it is worthwhile to develop organizational justice also to maintain cognitive working ability. Further longitudinal research is yet needed.
  • Pohjakallio, Katri (2023)
    Objectives: A student-athlete is a person who identifies him/herself as both an athlete and a student. Because both roles have demands, the demands overall can get high, which can predispose especially the young student-athletes to burnout symptoms. A strong identity can protect from burnout, but previous studies have had mixed results on the effect of the athletic identity to sport burnout and the association between student identity and school burnout has not yet been studied. The aim of this study was to evaluate how the strength of student and athletic identity are associated with school and sport burnout, and if there is an association between school and sport burnout in the population of student-athletes. Methods: The data used in this research were originally collected in the Winning in the Long Run-research project’s, a project studying student-athletes. The sample of this study consisted of 311 student-athletes. In this study, the information from the Winning In The Long Run- research project about the student-athletes sport burnout (SpBI-DC), school burnout (SBI), athletic identity (AIMS) and student identity (SIMS) was used. The relationships between sport burnout and athletic identity, school burnout and student identity and sport and school burnout were studied using regression analyses. Results: A stronger athletic identity was associated with fewer sport burnout symptoms, and a stronger student identity with fewer school burnout symptoms. In addition, it was observed that the more school burnout symptoms the student-athlete has, the more sport burnout symptoms she/he also experienced. Conclusions: The results suggest that a strong athletic identity might work as a protective factor from sport burnout symptoms for student-athletes and to consistently a strong student identity as a protective factor from school burnout. In the future, supporting the identity construction could be considered one way to prevent the student-athletes’ burnout symptoms in sport and school contexts. Because school and sport burnout were associated in this study, and studying and sports strongly intertwine in the lives of student-athletes, it raises a question of how possible it is for the student-athletes to distinguish the source of their burnout. In the future, it might be most beneficial to focus on supporting the student-athletes’ overall wellbeing in ensuring that there is enough support available for coping with the special demands of the dual career.
  • Plysjuk, Nadja (2023)
    Abstract Objectives of the study. Humans are adapted to recognize faces. Turning faces upside down causes an inversion effect that makes it much harder to recognize them. According to previous research, conflicting results have been obtained regarding which facial features are used the most in face recognition and short-term memory, and how turning the face upside down affects the use of features. It has also been unclear how turning the face upside down affects the use of these features. The aim of this thesis was to investigate, using a perceptual psychology experiment and a visual working memory task, whether different facial features are used to different extents in recognition and remembering and whether the facial features used differ in terms of whether the face is presented upright or upside down. Methods. The thesis used a new type of change detection test based on morphing. The material of the thesis consisted of ten students at the University of Helsinki. 40 neutral-looking faces from the Chicago Face Database served as face stimuli. Female and male face pairs were formed from the faces, whose facial features were morphed independently and randomly from 0–20% or 0–80% from the first identity to the second identity. The subject's task was to answer whether faces separated during the memory period, presented upright or upside down, were of the same or different identity. By comparing the number of morphs and the test subjects' answers with each other using a generalized linear model, weighting coefficients were obtained for each facial feature, which told how much the test subjects had used each feature in the test task. The weighting coefficients were normalized, after which they were subjected to a two-way analysis of variance of repeated measures, which was used to examine whether the use of features differed between the upright and upside down situations. In addition, possible differences in face recognition accuracy between upright and upside down situations were investigated using the discriminability index d'. Results and conclusions. According to the results, facial features differed in terms of how much they were used in the working memory task. However, the relative importance of different facial features was similar regardless of whether the face was presented upright or upside down. In line with previous findings, this study also found that the eyes are particularly significant in terms of face recognition and short-term memory. However, in the case of faces presented upside down, the identity recognition accuracy was weaker, which was reflected in a higher number of errors in the test task. In conclusion, turning faces upside down does not affect which facial features are stored in working memory. However, turning faces upside down weakens face recognition and short-term memory.
  • Kukkonen, Karoliina (2020)
    Objectives. Growth and fixed mindsets influence the performance in learning situations. This Master’s thesis investigated the manifestation of growth and fixed mindsets and the relationship of these beliefs with the ability to learn from mistakes and the amplitude difference of the error-related negativity (ERN) and correct response negativity (CRN) event-related potentials. This thesis investigated if a stronger growth mindset in one domain correlates with a stronger growth mindset in other domains compared to a fixed mindset. Furthermore, it was examined whether a growth mindset is associated with a smaller ERN – CRN amplitude difference and better post-error accuracy than a fixed mindset. Methods. The sample consisted of 97 8-11 year-old pupils. The electroencephalogram of the subjects was recorded during a mathematics task. In addition, post-error accuracy and growth and fixed mindsets of mathematical intelligence, general intelligence and emotion regulation were measured. Results and conclusions. A stronger growth mindset of mathematical intelligence correlated statistically significantly with a stronger growth mindset of general intelligence. The growth mindset of emotion regulation was not associated with that of mathematical or general intelligence. These three mindsets were not statistically significantly related to ERN – CRN amplitude difference or post-error accuracy. However, the relationship between a stronger growth mindset of mathematical intelligence and a smaller ERN – CRN amplitude difference approached significance. Moreover, against expectations a stronger growth mindset of emotion regulation was marginally significantly associated with smaller post-error accuracy. In line with earlier research, these results indicate that the growth mindsets of certain domains share some common variance. Inconsistent with previous studies, the effect of growth mindset on ERN – CRN amplitude difference or post-error accuracy could not be confirmed. This might be due to the young age of the participants and methodological challenges. This calls for further investigations of the maturation of growth and fixed mindsets and the ERN amplitude during different developmental stages.
  • Ritvanen, Noora (2022)
    Objective: Developmental coordination disorder (DCD) is a neurological disorder which affects motor function. According to studies with children DCD is related to volumes of the parts of the cerebrum which affect motor function. This study examines whether these structural changes in cerebrum can be detected in adults. Methods: The present study is a part of a longitudinal study which started in the 1970s in Helsinki. The present sample consists of those approximately 40-year-olds who took Test of Motor Impairment (TOMI) test when they were about 9 years old and who also participated in magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) study when they were about 40 years old. Based on TOMI test participants were split into two groups: those who probably have developmental coordination disorder and those who probably do not have it. Groups’ association with the volumes of cerebral areas that affect motor functions were examined with multivariate analyses of variance, one-way analyses of variance and Welch’s tests. The volumes of the examined parts of the brain were calculated from participants’ MRI data using FreeSurfer software, and the volumes were standardized according to gender before any analysis were made. Results and conclusion: The group had a statistically significant effect on the gross volumes of the brain areas and on the volumes of brain areas on the dominant hand side (areas which primarily affect movement of the nondominant side of the body) but not on the volumes of the brain areas on the nondominant hand side (areas which primarily affect movement of the dominant side of the body). In the group of those who probably have developmental coordination disorder the volume of the primary somatosensory cortex on the dominant hand side, volumes of the basal ganglia on the dominant and nondominant hand side and the volume of the backside of the corpus callosum were smaller than in the control group. The results suggest that about 40 years old adults who probably have developmental coordination disorder according to TOMI test have smaller volumes in parts of the cerebrum which affect motor function than adults who do not have the disorder.
  • Mäntylä, Tuuli (2022)
    Objectives: Developmental coordination disorder is a neurodevelopmental disorder with challenges in motor function, coordination, and balance. The etiology behind this disorder is still partly unknown. There has been a small number of studies made on the associations between developmental coordination disorder and brain structure and the results do not systematically reoccur. Thus, there is a need for more studies on the subject. In this study the objective was to examine the associations between probable developmental coordination disorder and cerebellum structure. Hypothesis was that there is a difference found between the study group and the control group in the volume of the cerebellum and that the volume is smaller in the probable developmental coordination disorder group. Methods: The present study’s data is from a prospective longitudinal cohort. The sample consisted of 295 participants of whom 22 had probable developmental coordination disorder. Motor function was assessed at the age of 9 with the Test of Motor Impairment, which was used to divide the participants in the probable developmental coordination disorder group and in the no developmental coordination disorder group. Magnetic resonance imaging (MRI) of the brain was completed at the age of 40. Cerebellum volumes were estimated using the software Ceres VolBrain. In the statistical analysis multivariate analysis of variance and analysis of variance were used. Results and conclusion: Probable developmental coordination disorder was not associated with the total volume of the cerebellum. However, there was a statistically significant association found with the volume of the cerebellum lobules III and V. In these lobules the volumes were smaller in the probable developmental coordination disorder group. The cerebellum is likely organized in a way that the areas behind motor function (lobules I–V) are different from the areas behind regulation of cognition (lobules VI– IX), possibly explaining the results of this study. The results indicate that developmental coordination disorder may occur in the structural level of the brain and provide information about the possible mechanisms behind the disorder. The present study provides a base for further studies to advance knowledge on the mechanisms behind developmental coordination disorder.
  • Arasalö, Martta (2021)
    Objectives: The well-being of young children’s parents has sparked interest in public discussion. Parents without a partner often experience higher distress compared to parents with a partner. Support from other people and family services might matter more for the former group. Social support’s effect on parental distress demands more research in Nordic welfare societies. The aim is to study Finnish mothers’ general stress and parenting demands, and their associations with partner status and support from other people and family services. Methods. The sample consisted of 5-year-old children’s mothers (n=714) participating in the CHILD SLEEP cohort study (THL). General stress and parenting demands were measured with the Perceived Stress Scale and the Parenting Stress Index. Mothers evaluated the amount and adequacy of childcare support from family, friends, and family services. Controlled variables were depression, anxiety, family structure and socioeconomic status. Linear regression models were used as analysis. Results. Most mothers reported moderate general stress and few experienced parenthood as demanding. Distress was not associated with partner status or the amount of support. Adequate support from family and friends was linked to lesser distress; similar indications were found with family service support. More mothers without a partner experienced inadequate support compared to those with a partner. Regardless of partner status lower distress was linked to adequate support, however associations were statistically significant more often among mothers with a partner. Conclusions. Preschool children’s mothers have moderate distress levels, and support from family and friends appears important for their well-being. Family service support requires more research. Similar distress levels regardless of partner status contradicts previous research, although the experience of insufficient support among mothers without a partner demands more investigation. Further research should use more representative samples regarding socioeconomic factors and family structure, and more varied measures of social support and partner status.
  • Metsola, Wilhelmiina (2022)
    Objective: Previous experimental and cross-sectional studies have found that paranormal thinking increases under stressful situations and is associated with retrospectively reported early and recent stress-prone life events. It has been suggested that paranormal thinking might act as a means to increase one’s sense of controllability and understandability of life events when being under burden. To date, no study has investigated the associations between exhaustion and paranormal thinking. Therefore, the present study aimed to 1) examine the cross-sectional associations between exhaustion and paranormal thinking, 2) examine whether exhaustion predicts the development of paranormal thinking over an 11-year follow-up, and 3) investigate whether different developmental trajectories of exhaustion are differently associated with paranormal thinking. Methods: The participants (n = 906–2092) came from The Young Finns Study, which is a Finnish population-based on-going follow-up study consisting of six age cohorts (born between 1962 and 1977). Paranormal thinking was assessed in 2001 and 2012 with The Spiritual Acceptance vs. Rational Materialism questionnaire. Exhaustion was assessed in 2001, 2007 and 2012 with The Maastricht Vital Exhaustion questionnaire. The participants were 24-50-year-olds during the measurement years. The cross-sectional and longitudinal associations between exhaustion and paranormal thinking were examined using linear regression analyses. The analyses were adjusted for participants’ gender and age, and participants’ and their parents’ socioeconomic status. The associations of different developmental trajectories of vital exhaustion (i.e., consistently high, consistently low, increasing, or decreasing exhaustion over the 11-year follow-up) with paranormal thinking were examined using analyses of variance. Results and conclusion: The results of this study showed that high exhaustion was cross-sectionally associated with higher levels of paranormal thinking. Further, high vital exhaustion predicted higher development of paranormal thinking over the 11-year follow-up. Additionally, participants with consistently high exhaustion over the 11-year follow-up had higher levels of paranormal thinking, when compared to those without exhaustion or those with increasing levels of exhaustion over the 11-year follow-up. The findings provide novel evidence on the associations between exhaustion and paranormal thinking that had not been investigated before. Future research could examine whether paranormal thinking could act as a psychological coping strategy among exhausted individuals.
  • Heikkinen, Tuukka (2023)
    Background and aims. Using computers is an almost inseparable part of everyday life today, and problems with computer use have tangible and significant consequences for quality of life and societal functioning. Performance in everyday computer use is affected by a multitude of factors such as the usability of computer systems and interfaces, the age of the user, prior computer use experience and cognitive abilities. The aim of this study was to examine how cognitive abilities, such as verbal comprehension, perceptual reasoning, working memory and processing speed affect everyday computer use. Methods. The data consisted of 88 participants (37 men and 51 women), who were actively employed and between 20-65 years of age. In the study each participant performed two parts lasting a maximum of 2 hours on separate days. The first part consisted of computer tasks simulating everyday computer use, and the second part was an assessment of cognitive abilities using the WAIS-IV. The data was analyzed using hierarchical linear regression on three outcome variables: completeness of the computer tasks, completion time and the mental load caused by them. In the regression models each outcome variable was explained with sociodemographic factors, prior computer use experience, cognitive abilities and executive functions. Results and conclusions. Sociodemographic factors explained 33-51% of computer task performance on all the outcome variables, and the strongest predictor was age. Computer use experience explained 6-13% of task completeness and completion time, but did not significantly affect mental load. Cognitive abilities explained 4-10% of all the outcome variables, and the strongest predictor was Full Scale IQ. Executive functions explained 3-4% of task completeness and mental load, but did not significantly affect completion time. This study indicates that the cognitive abilities of the user and their computer use experience clearly affect everyday computer use. The results can be utilized in the development of computer systems and interfaces: ideally using them would be easy and intuitive enough that the level of the user's cognitive abilities and their prior computer use experience would not matter for their successful use.
  • Pöntinen, Janna (2021)
    Objectives. It is important to identify the factors behind stress at work, because stress can impair health and job performance. Based on previous studies, there may be a wide range of factors behind stress such as job demands, interpretation of situations and job autonomy. The aim of this study was to examine the associations between cognitive demands, autonomy and stress in teaching. Methods. The data was gathered by the Finnish Institute of Occupational Health’s Brain Work Survey. Participants (n = 424) worked in a teaching organization. The survey assessed the prevalence of cognitive demands at a weekly level, as well as the perceived load and perceived inspiring cognitive demands. Autonomy was assessed by exploring the opportunities participants have for influencing factors relevant to the performance of their work. The perceived stress was assessed on a numerical scale from zero to ten. Hierarchical regression analysis was used for the statistical analysis of the data. Results and conclusions. Cognitive demands and autonomy were associated with stress. Higher amount of instruction problems and perceived cognitive load were associated to higher perceived stress. The more autonomy there was at work, the less stress was experienced. However, the explanation rate of autonomy was closely related to cognitive demands. The results can be utilized in developing work as favourable for health and performance as possible.
  • Mäntylä, Paula (2020)
    Aims of the study Many changes have been made to the school environment in Finland. Still there are no research about how new school environments will effect pupils’ well-being, such as self-esteem. Due to the lack of previous research it cannot be concluded whether these changes to school environment are equal to every student regardless of their temperament. The aim of this study is to find out about the effects of the lack of stability in the school environment (stability of social relations, stability of physical features, stability of overall structure) to pupils’ self-esteem. In addition, the possible moderating effect of temperament is under an interest. Methods The representative sample of this study (n=2210) consisted of Finnish 9th graders, who participated in The Finnish Study on Temperament and School Achievement –research project between 2005 and 2006. Self-esteem was measured by the Finnish version of the Coopersmith Self-Esteem Inventory. Temperament traits were measured by TABC-R and DOTS-R –inventories. The stability of the school environment was measured with three different scales: the stability of social relations, physical features and overall structure. The study was carried out by using multilevel models. Three multilevel models were created, in each of which self-esteem was the dependent variable. Results and conclusions The stability of social relations, physical features or overall structure did not have statistically significant main effects on the pupils’ self-esteem. However, the non-steadiness of overall structure had a statistically significant negative effect on the self-esteem of pupils with a higher task orientation. Reactivity or flexibility did not moderate any effects of the stability scales. All the observed effects were small, which implies that there are a number of different variables not measured in this study that affect pupils’ self-esteem. The results of this study show that the non-steady school environments do not have a positive effect on pupils’ well-being, as it has been assumed. The non-steadiness of the school environment with other changes in the school environment and their effects to pupils’ well-being need further research.
  • Kukkonen, Saara (2020)
    Aims: Previous research has shown that high instability of home, work, or daycare environment is associated with poorer cognitive achievement. This topic, however, has not been investigated in school environment. This study investigated (1) whether instability of school environment is associated with students’ school achievement and (2) whether students’ temperament traits modify these associations. Method: The data included a representative sample of Finnish 9th grade students (N=1835–1915). The instability of school environment was measured with three instability scales: instability of social relationships, learning spaces, and general structure. School achievement was measured with school grades. Standardized cognitive tests were performed for a subsample of the data (N=121–137). Students’ temperament traits were assessed using questionnaires fulfilled by teachers. Results: High instability of social relationships was related to poorer school grades independently of student’s temperament traits. High instability of learning spaces was associated with poorer grades in students with low (vs. high) task orientation or high (vs. low) flexibility. Instability of school environment had no main effects to standardized cognitive tests. High instability of learning spaces was associated with poorer cognitive achievement in students with high task orientation (vs. low) or high reactivity (vs. low). Conclusions: High instability of school environment appears to be associated with students’ poorer school achievement. These associations are modified by students’ temperament traits. The results indicate that there should not be excessive instability of students’ social relationships or learning spaces to provide optimal and equal opportunities for school achievement for students with different temperaments.
  • Taulavuori, Sonja (2020)
    Objectives. As the population is aging, it is important to support healthy aging. Active musical hobbies, like choir singing, are a promising tool for this. So far, there are only a few controlled studies that explore the benefits of choir singing. The aim of this study was to investigate how choir singing is associated with social support, mood and cognitive performance. In addition, the association between choir singing and musical engagement as well as the associations between musical engagement and social support and mood were examined. Methods. Healthy seniors (N=105) aged 60 years and older (mean age 71 years) participated in this cross-sectional study. Of the subjects, 61 subjects were choir singers and 44 were controls. A majority (70 %) of the subjects were female with, on average, short-cycle tertiary level education. Perceived social support, mood and musical engagement were assessed with questionnaires. General cognition, executive function, working memory and verbal short-term memory were assessed with neuropsychological tests. The association of choir singing with social support, mood, cognitive performance and musical engagement were examined with analyses of covariance (ANCOVA). Associations of musical engagement with social support and mood were examined with correlation analyses. Results. Compared to the control group, the choir singers had higher scores on the Social Provisions Scale, especially in general social support, attachment, social integration and guidance, but not in reassurance of worth, reliable alliance or opportunity for nurturance. There was no difference between the groups on mood or cognitive performance. The choir singers also reported higher musical engagement on almost all scales of the Music Engagement Questionnaire compared to the controls. Musical engagement was associated especially with general social support, social integration and reassurance of worth. Conclusion. This study supports and elaborates on previous evidence of social benefits of choir singing, but not emotional or cognitive benefits. Social characteristics of choir singing are a major advantage compared to other leisure activities. Instead, choir singing does not stand out from other leisure activities with its emotional and cognitive qualities. This study addresses the close relationship between choir singing and musical engagement which in turn, is associated with social support. These findings can be utilized to support healthy aging and in prevention, for example, of loneliness.
  • Turkkila, Roosa (2023)
    The purpose of this study was to examine how inverting the face upside down impacts the perception of audiovisual speech and especially the integration of sensory information during such speech perception. It is well known that face perception involves specialized mechanisms, and it has been suggested that inverting the face would impair the use of to these mechanisms. On the other hand, speech perception is also known to be a specialized process, categorized by a strong audiovisual summation. The aim of this study was to find out whether the strong integration of sensory information in the perception of audiovisual speech is one such specialized mechanism that impairs when the face is inverted. The potential difference in audiovisual summation between upright and inverted faces was investigated using subthreshold summation paradigm. In this paradigm, single and multisensory discrimination thresholds were measured by presenting subjects video clips of spoken syllables [pa] and [ka] masked with audiovisual noise. The task was to identify which of the syllables were presented. Then, by fitting the Minkowski metric to the separation thresholds it is possible to derive different summation models that describe the strength of summation. The study’s results suggest that visual speech perception was poorer when the faces were perceived upside down. However, the audiovisual summation was equally strong for both upright and inverted faces. These results align with the previous knowledge that visual speech perception is considerably impaired when the face is inverted. While there is limited previous research on the effect of inversion on audiovisual summation using a similar setup, this study indicates that auditory and visual speech are combined in the same manner for both upright and inverted faces. Therefore, it seems, that the mechanisms responsible for the strong summation taken place in audiovisual speech perception appear to be distinct from those processes that are impaired in face inversion.