Skip to main content
Login | Suomeksi | På svenska | In English

Browsing by discipline "Psykologia"

Sort by: Order: Results:

  • Paunonen, Jarno (2016)
    The purpose of this study was to examine the possible connection of applicants' self-esteem and gender to their performance in student selection for teacher education. The significance of teacher's personality has grown with working conditions that increasingly demand better social skills. Teachers have reported unusual tiredness and the willingness to quit has become more common. It seems reasonable to consider self-esteem as a protective buffer against the emotional stress these working conditions present for teachers, and therefore the student selection at least shouldn't favor those with whom this protective buffer is low. Self-esteem's connection with student selection for teacher education has not been studied before, but due to prior results regarding the broader benefits of high self-esteem, it was hypothesized to be positively connected to success in student selection. The selection process consists of a theory test and an aptitude test, which were addressed separately and together. In line with prior research, women were hypothesized to be more successful in theory test and men in the aptitude test. This study was a part of the SeSTE research project (Selecting Students for Teacher Education). There were 470 subjects who had participated in the selection process for teacher education in the University of Helsinki. Self-esteem was measured with Rosenberg self-esteem scale as self-assessment. The main method of analyzing was multinomial logistic regression. Gender was examined alongside self-esteem, and the effect of age was controlled. The hypotheses for self-esteem were mostly confirmed with no apparent connection seen with the aptitude test. Gender related hypotheses were confirmed as women were more likely to pass the theory test and men the aptitude test. According to this study the student selection slightly favors applicants with higher self-esteem, but the effect is small. If the hypothesis of high self-esteem as a useful protective buffer for teachers gains more evidence, it might be beneficial according to this study, to consider the possibility of focusing the student selection more to applicants with higher self-esteem.
  • Vuori, Outi (2014)
    Test anxiety is related to impaired performance in different cognitive tasks. Relation between test anxiety and performance has been found mainly in mathematical, visual and working memory tasks but also in overall performance in WAIS-III -version. This Master's thesis examines the relation between test anxiety and cognitive task performance encompassing in WAIS-IV -test. Sample benefits WAIS-IV's Finnish standardization sample. The test anxiety estimation was taken in 599 cases by the researcher and in 611 cases by self evaluation. 262 (44 %) of those evaluated by the researcher experienced little, medium or a lot of anxiety. 299 (49 %) of the self evaluation examinees experienced little, medium or a lot of anxiety. Test anxiety was noticed to be related to cognitive performance in subtests, indices and overall performance. Test anxiety was evaluated by the researcher and was noticed to be related to subtests that measure working memory, perceptual reasoning and processing speed negatively. Test anxiety evaluated by the researcher was related to all indices where as self reported test anxiety was related to only working memory index and perceptual reasoning index. As evaluated by the researcher more differences were found both in subtests and in the indices than the results of self reported test anxiety. Regardless of the evaluation (by the researcher or self reported) test anxiety was related to both Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) and General Ability Index (GAI). Based on this Master's thesis it can be said that test anxiety has a comprehensively negative relation to cognitive test performance. Test anxiety weakens individual's test performance and abilities to adduce thoroughly which sets a risk to misinterpretation and underestimation of one's abilities in evaluating cognitive capacity in clinical work. Therefore attention should be paid to test anxiety in test situations.
  • Pakkanen, Anna (2015)
    Objectives. Earlier studies have shown intergenerational continuity in child-rearing attitudes. The child-rearing attitudes reflect the atmosphere of childhood. There is only a few longitudinal studies of this topic and temperament has not been studied earlier in this context. The purpose of this study was to examine the intergenerational transmission of parental child-rearing attitudes (i.e. parent's perception of emotional significance and tolerance towards her offspring) using longitudinal data. Also, the role of sociability as a mediating factor for the child-rearing attitudes was examined. There were two hypothesis of the study: There is intergenerational continuity in child-rearing attitudes, and sociability mediates the relationship between child-rearing attitudes of the two generations. Methods. The participants of this study (N=1034) were derived from the ongoing prospective Cardiovascular Risk in Young Finns study. It was started in 1980, when the participants were 3-18 years old. The subsample of this study consisted of six age cohorts (63,2 % women) and their mothers. The self-rated child-rearing attitudes towards the offspring were measured in 1980 by the participants' mothers and 2007 by the participants. The sociability temperament trait was measured by the participants themselves in 1992 at the age from 15 to 30. Linear regression analysis was used to examine the relationships between sociability and child-rearing attitudes. Sobel test was also performed to examine the mediating role of sociability. Results and conclusions. In line with the hypothesis, the child-rearing attitudes were transferred from the older generation to the younger one. The intergenerational transmission was stronger with emotional significance and weaker with tolerance towards the offspring. Sociability did not act as a mediating factor in either one of the child-rearing attitudes. However, sociability was associated with parent's perception of emotional significance of the offspring. The more sociable the participant was at the age of 15-30, the more emotionally significant he/she perceived his/her own offspring in 2007, 15 years later. The interventions that support parenting have long-term effects for future generations because the child-rearing attitudes are intergenerationally transferred.
  • Orenius, Olli (2015)
    People tend to first look evenly at both objects when they are making a decision between them. Gaze starts to get directed more to the object which is eventually chosen before selecting it consciously. One explanation for the phenomenon is the gaze cascade model, which states, that directing of gaze is related to making decisions based on preference. It also states that the gaze bias is influenced by cognitive models people have about the perceived stimulus. Gaze bias should be greater the less one has previous experience, i.e. cognitive models about the objects. This study evaluates these two assumptions by the gaze cascade model. 64 subjects participated in the experiment from which 54 subjects' data was used in the final analysis (average age 27.7, range 18–47 years). Stimuli consisted from images of cheese packages which are sold in Finland, images of cheese packages sold abroad and abstract images. The assumption was that the subjects would have most cognitive models about the packages sold in Finland, the second most about the packages sold abroad and least about the abstract images. Subjects made choices about the stimuli by preference, size and ecology. Direction of gaze during decision making was recorded with gaze tracking goggles. Likelihood that the subjects were looking at the stimulus which they chose was estimated for 53 sampling points, 1.77 seconds before the conscious decision. Likelihood scores were fitted to sigmoid functions by least square method. The amount of gaze bias during different decision making instructions and stimuli was compared with a two sample Kolmogorov-Smirnov test. Gaze bias was greater the less subjects had previous experience about the stimuli. Large gaze bias was perceived also in other tasks than preference selection. For this reason gaze cascade model does not seem to provide a good explanation for gaze bias during decision making. There was also found an interaction between the used decision criteria and stimulus type. Earlier studies have usually focused on examining either the effect of the decision criteria or the used stimulus type to the gaze bias. This study shows that the interaction between decision criteria and stimulus type should be taken into consideration when examining the gaze bias during decision making. One possible explanation for the interaction might be the difficultness of the choice. Gaze bias during decision making might be especially related to situations where the difference between two stimuli evaluated by given criteria is very small.
  • Viertola, Hannele (2015)
    Concentration as a cognitive function is closely related to attention which has many definitions. Functional subsystems of attention are crucial in cognitive processing and attention is related to cognitive performance. The Wechsler Adult Intelligence Scale (WAIS) is a test designed to measure cognitive performance and the newest, fourth edition of the test (WAIS-IV) has also been standardized in Finland. Because attention is crucial in cognitive processing it is relevant to examine the association between concentration difficulties and cognitive task performance in WAIS-IV –test. This Master's thesis is based on WAIS-IV's Finnish standardization sample. Observation of concentration difficulties by the examiner was available in 600 cases and self-evaluation by the examinee in 607 cases. In 580 cases both evaluations were available. Based on examiner observation 358 (59,7%) of subjects experienced little, medium or a lot of concentration difficulties. Based on self-evaluation 419 (69%) of the examinees experienced little, medium or a lot of concentration difficulties. Differences between the evaluations were found in distributions of concentration difficulties groups and in relations between concentration difficulties and cognitive task performance in WAIS-IV –test. Concentration difficulties evaluated by the researcher were related to almost all subtests, all indices, General Ability Index (GAI) and to Full Scale IQ (FSIQ) where as self reported concentration difficulties were not related to cognitive task performance in WAIS-IV –test. The results show that concentration difficulties are common and they may have an overall negative relation to cognitive test performance. One of the main purposes in clinical neuropsychological examination is to evaluate the optimal cognitive performance level of the examinee. For this reason it is crucial to pay attention to negative relation of concentration difficulties to cognitive test performance. This improves the reliability of the clinical evaluation of cognitive performance.
  • Lappalainen, Petra-Sif Markkusdottir (2015)
    Objective. International adoption concerns large groups of children each year. International adoptees often have developmental delays or deficits in the social and physical domains due to depriving early living conditions. The atypical development of attachment has been of special interest in the population of adopted children. The purpose of the present study is to explore the possible connections between an adoptee's background factors and the symptoms or behaviors indicating disordered attachment. This study is part of the ongoing Finnish Adoption Study. Procedure. The chosen background variables were adoptee's gender, continent of origin, the number and form of placements before adoption, and adoptee's age at adoption. Symptoms of disordered attachment were evaluated with a questionnaire developed for this study. The items represented the two types of attachment disorder, disinhibited and inhibited, defined in the DSM-IV-TR and ICD-10. In addition, two items concerned clingy behavior. Adoptive parents filled out the questionnaire as a part of a larger questionnaire concerning the background and health of the adoptee in general (n=1450). The questionnaire was mailed to all the parents who had adopted a child internationally between the years 1985 and 2007. The relationships between the variables were investigated using linear regression and two-way analysis of variance. Results and conclusions. The results showed that adoptee's gender, continent of origin and adoptee's age at adoption were connected to disinhibited symptoms. Living in a single orphanage and the continents Asia and Africa were related to inhibited symptoms. Only Asia of the explored variables was related to clingy behavior. Based on the results it was concluded that different kind of attachment related behavior problems might have different kind of developmental paths. The results of this study can be utilized in future research to find out more detailed information about the development of disordered attachment behavior in adopted children. The results are important for the provision of well-informed information to adoptive parents.
  • Helander, Harriet Erica (2016)
    Attachment relationships to primary caregivers are one of the key aspects that make a difference when considering small children's overall emotional development. The effects of attachment have been studied widely and they have been acknowledged to have great influence in children's socio emotional development. Concurrently with the primary attachment relationships formed at home a child can have secondary attachment relationships with people who regularly take care of them. So children might also form secondary attachments to day care nurses. In Finland 63% of all under school aged children and 53% of all two year olds attend to day care. Full-time day care is physiologically stressful for most toddlers and early commenced long days in day care outside the home can predict anxious, unadapted and aggressive behaviour to rise in later years. All this taken into account it is obvious that the early care in day care and attachment relationships within are very meaningful to little toddlers. Based on scientific literature we know that by focusing on caregivers' sensitivity, homelike care environments and high quality of the overall care we can support the formation of secure attachment relationships and reduce the stressfulness of day care. In Helsinki University's Kenguru- project a primary nursing design was made to assist the actualization of these factors. This study is a part of that project. The aim of this study was to investigate if having a primary nurse design in day care will promote secure attachments towards the primary nurses. The hypothesis was that primary nursing design will increase attachment formation in day care. It was assumed that the children who got a primary nurse to look after them would make stronger bonds toward their caregiver than the children who didn't have a primary nurse to care for them. Attachment relationships were rated twice during the year with the Attachment Q-sort that has been found to be a reliable measure of attachment between a dyad. Study group consisted of 97 children in 14 different day care groups. The results showed that the primary nurse design did not have an attachment promoting effect throughout the whole group of children. When the subgroups were studied further one could see that it was the subgroup of the old children who had already been to day care that had a decrease in their attachment. All other subgroups had attachment relationship scores staying the same throughout the year. The results shown in this study are largely limited as there were only two groups where the primary nurse design could be executed as planned. Nevertheless this study gave some reference that small children under the age of three could really benefit a primary nurse design and being cared for in a smaller group. This supports the notion that children under the age of three could benefit homelike day care in smaller groups. Further studies are needed to really comprehend the influences of primary nurse design to children's socioemotional development in the long run.
  • Jaalas, Anni (2016)
    Introduction. Cognitive performance and school engagement have been shown to affect school performance from childhood through adolescence to adulthood. Higher working memory performance and attentional skills can predict success in many different school subjects, such as math, English and science. Individual differences in executive attention have also been shown to predict the length of education from childhood to adulthood. Studies have also shown that motivational characteristics such as school engagement can predict school achievements and the years of schooling attained. Only a few studies have tried to investigate the joint effects that cognitive skills and motivational characteristics have on school performance. This study aims to examine how cognitive skills (working memory and attention) and school engagement jointly affect school performance in Finnish adolescent students. One aim was also to find out if school engagement mediates the connection between cognitive performance and school performance. Methods. The current study included 105 volunteering adolescents (53 females) who participated in the Mind the Gap (2012 – 2016) study that was funded by the Academy of Finland. The study included two age groups: 13–14 year olds (n=50) and 16–17 year olds (n=55). Participants performed tasks that measured working memory performance and executive attention and they filled out a questionnaire that included the school engagement inventory. School achievement was evaluated with both official grades and students self-assessed grades. Structural equation modelling was used to analyze the relationships between the examined variables. Results and conclusions. The study showed that both cognitive performance and school engagement predicted school performance. Especially the connections between self-assessed grades and cognitive performance and school engagement were more significant than connections with official grades. This study did not find a significant relationship between cognitive performance and school engagement, which means that there was no significant mediation between cognitive performance, school engagement and school performance. This study provides further scientific evidence that cognitive performance and school engagement both affect school performance. Even though cognitive skills and school engagement were not significantly related in this study, it would be important to conduct more research on this topic in the future.
  • Valtola, Merja (2015)
    The aim of this current study is to increase awareness and to provide insight into the human psychological constraints and their contribution on hazardous situations at work as well as to provide new tools for the further development of occupational safety. Interruptions at work are common and may result in hazardous situations. The accident risk caused by interruption is often based on the employee's work experience converted into automatic cognitive processing. We know very little about the impact of interruptions on the automatic processing. In addition to other adverse effects it may cause, interruption may result also in cognitive bias. Interruption may lead to a situation, where we falsely believe that what has been started, has also been done. The case study used data that has been screened in international and national fatality investigation databases. The selected cases were analysed by applying a cognitive analysis of interruption method. From the available literature, research and analysis of the cases, it appears that the automatic cognitive processing is very vulnerable to interruptions. Vulnerability was manifested especially in situations where the attention demanding controlled processing broke off automatic performance. Automatic processing can also lead to situations with significant risk when unexpected change in the environment is occurred or if environmental conditions no longer reflect the current automatic cognitive processing. In automatic work process, which includes subtasks operated always in same specific order, case analysis gives a clue to the emergence of cognitive bias. This is manifested in a situation where already started subtask or part of the started work, which is perceived as a whole, is interrupted. However, more research is needed to determine the exact mechanisms behind this phenomenon. The damage interruptions might cause during an automatic work performance can be reduced by the use of external cognitive tools to increase attention and to ensure situational awareness especially when disengaging and/or resuming the original task and by the development of technical security solutions.
  • Hirvelä, Satu (2016)
    Objective: Depression and anxiety disorders are mood disorders which may result from a number of psychological, social and biological reasons. Dysregulation of HPA-axis, such as hypercortisolism and hypocortisolism, is thought to be connected to depression and anxiety. On the other hand depression and anxiety are also connected to the personality characteristics like high neuroticism. The aim of this study was to examine the connections of personality characteristics and evening cortisol to depression, anxiety and their comorbidity. These have not been previously studied together. Methods: This study used data from the second wave of the MIDUS (Midlife in the United States) longitudinal study. The data was collected by the Institute on Aging at the University of Wisconsin-Madison during 2004-2006. Personality characteristics were assessed by the short personality scale of MIDUS, where respondents assessed the suitability of 25 adjectives to themselves in a four-step scale. Depression and anxiety were measured by MASQ (Mood and Anxiety Symptom Questionnaire) which had been modified for MIDUS. Cortisol was measured from saliva at four different time points during four days. ANOVA, linear regression and multi-nominal logistic regression were used for data analysis. Results and conclusions: Low evening cortisol level appears to be predictive of anhedonic depression in low educated young people. The personality characteristics of high neuroticism and low conscientiousness predicted all symptom groups, which is in line with previous studies. High neuroticism was the biggest risk for comorbid depression and anxiety. In addition to high neuroticism and low conscientiousness, female sex, middle and low level education also predicted somatic anxiety and hypocortisolism, female sex, low extraversion and middle and low level education predicted anhedonic depression. Furthermore, a low level of education was positively associated with comorbidity. Neuroticism is a risk for mood disorders and understanding its development in childhood requires further research. Neuroticism should be taken in account in clinical practice. Psychotherapy might be effective to reduce neuroticism.
  • Myllymäki, Outi (2015)
    Objectives. Previous research has pointed out various predictors of school achievement. Along with cognitive capacity, school achievement is associated with female gender, beneficial socioeconomic status, school temperament and school motivation. The aim of this study is to bring together various findings of previous research of qualities influencing school achievement, and to find out whether school motivation is connected with school grades after controlling for pupil's gender and self-rated temperament. It is important to thoroughly understand school motivation, because among other things it has also been associated with effective use of learning strategies. Through vaster understanding of school motivation it is possible to find ways to motivate pupils in risk of social exclusion. Methods. This research is based on a vast Finnish SITRA school study. This study uses a subsample of 3 040 pupils between ages 13 and 19 from 64 different schools forming a geographically representative sample of Finnish speaking ninth graders. Pupils provided self-rated answers for temperament surveys DOTS-R and TABC-R as well as to school motivation survey created for the use of the SITRA project. Their latest grade point average was also self-reported. The data was analyzed using linear regression analysis. Results and conclusions. As the main result, school motivation explained 38 % of the variation of school grades, when the pupil's gender and self-rated temperament were controlled. Girls got better school grades than boys. However in regards to school motivation, there was no difference between the genders. There were differences between temperament traits between boys and girls, however ideal school temperament traits were divided to both genders evenly. All school temperament traits were correlated with school motivation. The strongest positive connection was found with persistence and the strongest negative connection with impulsivity. There may be a common factor, school environment for example, explaining the strong correlation between school temperament and school motivation. All results are in line with previous evidence.
  • Laamanen, Petra (2015)
    There are relatively few studies on pre-attentive auditory processing in middle-aged people. However, previous studies have shown that aging affects the ability to detect changes in regular auditory input at pre-attentive level as well as to involuntary allocation of attention. Recording of event-related brain potentials (ERPs) provides a good way for examining these phenomena. One purpose of this study was to find out whether musical expertise effects pre-attentive auditory processing in this particular age group. Results of many experiments have shown that children and young adults with musical expertise are more sensitive to acoustic properties of musical and phonemic sounds. There is also some evidence that adult musicians detect deviant pure tones more accurately than non-musicians. Based on this, we hypothesized that musicians would discriminate deviant tones in this study more accurately as well. Second aim of this study was to find out whether multi-feature paradigm can be used to examine pre-attentive auditory processing in middle-aged participants. In previous studies with young adults the multi-feature paradigm has proven to be a suitable way to study short-term memory and attention allocation. The 24 participants were derived into two groups based on their level of musical expertise. Participants in music group practiced music regularly and participants in non-music group had some other free time activities. We used a multi-feature paradigm that consisted of pure tone sound sequence in which four types of acoustic changes (frequency, duration, intensity and perceived sound-source location) varied in every other tone in three deviation magnitude (small, medium and large). Based on previous studies we assumed that these deviant tones would elicit both mismatch negativity (MMN) and P3a components, which are thought to reflect automatic, pre-attentive auditory processing. The magnitude of deviation was presumed to reflect in MMN and P3a amplitudes. In this study, no between-group differences were found for MMN or P3a amplitudes. However, MMN distributions slightly differed both frontally and laterally in these two groups. This finding might indicate that musical expertise has an influence on which parts of the brain auditory input is processed. As expected, deviant tones of small, medium and large magnitude elicited MMN components and medium and large deviations elicited also P3a components. As a rule, the amplitude of components increased with the magnitude of deviance. These results are in line with previous studies and show that the multi-feature paradigm can be used to examine pre-attentive auditory processing in middle-aged as in younger adults.
  • Tuominen, Mirka (2016)
    Objectives. Anxiety disorders and anxiety symptoms are common even as a child, and they cause a major burden to the child as well as to the society. In order to prevent anxiety disorders it is important to understand the predisposing factors to anxiety. The influence of child's temperament and parenting style on children's anxiety symptoms have both been studied, but the results are partly controversial, and there's only a limited number of longitudinal studies. The importance of interactions between temperament and parenting style has been emphasized, but the amount of interaction studies is scarce. The aim of this study is to examine the associations between child's temperament and anxiety symptoms, associations between parenting style and child's anxiety symptoms, and whether parenting style is a moderator between the association with temperament and anxiety symptoms. Methods. The sample consisted of 262 mother-child dyads participating in the Glychyrrhizin in Licorice-study. Mothers assessed their child's temperament with the Children's Behavior Questionnaire and their parenting style with the Parent Behavior Inventory when the children were 5,5 years old. Children assessed their anxiety symptoms with the Screen for Child Anxiety Related Disorders when they were 12 years old. The associations between the variables were analyzed using regression analyzes. Results and conclusions. High negative emotionality and its subfactors anger, fear, sadness and low soothability were associated with several anxiety symptoms. Extraversion was not associated with any anxiety symptoms, but its subfactor high impulsivity predicted increased anxiety symptoms. Low effortful control and its subfactor low inhibitory control were associated with symptoms of school phobia, and low intensity pleasure was associated with panic symptoms. Anxiety symptoms were associated with hostile/coercive parenting style, but not with supportive/engaged parenting style. Both parenting styles moderated some of the associations between temperament traits and anxiety symptoms. The findings give support both to the independent effect of temperament to children's anxiety symptoms and interactions between temperament and parenting. The findings give also some preliminary support to the "goodness of fit" –model, indicating that the effect of parenting style on children's anxiety symptoms may depend on child's temperament.
  • Elo, Heini (2016)
    Temperament is child's individual characteristic, and it serves as a core to personality development. Still we do not have a clear understanding how stable temperament traits are from infancy onward. A strong attachment bond to caregivers is important to child's development. Attachment bond is known to form also in secondary relationships as with professional caregivers. Temperament is associated with attachment security in primary attachment relationship. Nevertheless, studies on associations with child temperament and quality of secondary attachment are few and far between. This study will focus on the questions about (1) stability of child temperament, (2) association between child temperament and security of attachment with professional caregiver, and (3) how security of attachment will mediate the stability of child temperament, diminishing or strengthening temperament traits. This study is part of Kenguru-project in which professional caregiving was examined in day care centers concerning children under three years old during 2005 - 2006. In day care centers we were running an intervention to boost interaction and development of attachment bond between children and caregivers. This study examined those 126 children (girls 54) who were 8 - 34 months (mean age 23 months) old in the beginning of the study. Temperament was assessed as emotionality, activity and shyness, and we used EAS - temperament questionnaire (parental ratings), at two time points. Security of attachment was observed using Attachment Q-sort. Among other research questions, the factor structure of EAS-temperament questionnaire was observed. The stability of temperament was studied with correlations coefficients and all research questions were analysed using the linear regression modelling. According to this study, there were some changes in child temperament in the short term. Most changes occurred in emotionality. This result support the earlier finding that temperamental traits will be more stable when child get older than in infancy or in toddlerhood. There were no association between emotionality, activity or shyness with the security of attachment. Findings are in line with earlier research, that parental ratings about child temperament are not associated with observed attachment security with professional caregiver.
  • Kallinen, Jutta (2016)
    Objectives. Theory of mind is the ability to assign mental states (e.g. beliefs, intentions and desires) to oneself and others as well as to recognise that others have mental states that differ from one's own. Theory of mind helps individuals understand others' minds and interpret and predict their behaviour in relation to their mental states. Thus, it is one of the most important skills for children's social development. Normally developing children are usually able to make inferences about the mental states of other people by 5 years of age. Theory of mind development is associated with e.g. verbal ability, gender, culture and family background. Recent studies indicate that temperament may also have an influence on children's theory of mind. Specifically, a less reactive, more observant temperament seems to enhance theory of mind development. Poor theory of mind may lead to various difficulties in social relationships. Thus, it is important to investigate relations between temperament and theory of mind. The aim of this study was to examine if children's temperament at 9 months and 3 years predicts theory of mind at 5 years of age. Gender differences in the relationship between temperament and theory of mind were also investigated. Method. This study used a sample from the prospective British Millennium Cohort Study. Child temperament was assessed by parental reports at 9 months and 3 years of age. Theory of mind was assessed with a false belief task at 5 years of age. The relations between child temperament and theory of mind were examined using logistic regression (n=8041). Results and conclusions. Child temperament at age 9 months did not predict theory of mind at age 5. However, low hyperactivity and inattention, high independence and self regulation, and high prosocial behaviour at age 3 predicted theory of mind at age 5 after controlling for gender, ethnicity, vocabulary and maternal education. There were no significant gender differences in the relations between temperament and theory of mind. In accord with previous studies, these findings suggest a relation between individual differences in temperament and theory of mind development. Future research is needed to clarify relations between temperament and theory of mind during infancy and early childhood.
  • Lahtiharju, Elina (2015)
    Earlier studies have found some evidence of associations between childhood noncompliance and externalizing problems. The association of noncompliance with internalizing problems is instead less studied. In both cases the results have been controversial and are based on relatively small samples. The purpose of this study was to examine the association of lower childhood compliance with later-onset externalizing and internalizing behavior problems. Further, the study examined the association of child's cognitive skills and mother's socioeconomic status (income and educational level) with behavior problems, and their interaction effects with compliance. The sample consisted of children and their mothers from the National Longitudinal Study of Youth. The subjects belonged to three different groups based on their follow-up lengths; the follow-ups varied between 0-2 years (n=787), 4 years (n=1441) and 6-10 years (n=1569). Statistical analyses were conducted using linear regression model. The models were built separately for each follow-up group. The results showed that lower compliance predicted overall amount of behavior problems, externalizing and internalizing problems on every follow-up. Child's cognitive skills also predicted externalizing and internalizing problems. Mother's level of income or education did not predict behavior problems. However, the level of income moderated the association between compliance and externalizing problems on the shortest follow-up. Also the mother's educational level moderated the association between compliance and externalizing problems on the shortest follow-up and internalizing problems on the longest follow-up. The results provide support for previous theoretical models examining the association between compliance and behavior problems. Moreover, the results emphasize the role of both individual (e.g., compliance and cognitive skills) and environmental factors (e.g., socioeconomic status) in the etiology of behavior problems.
  • Fagerlund, Hanne (2016)
    Objectives. Late-preterm birth (34+0-36+6 weeks' gestation) has been associated with increased risk of adverse outcomes in executive functioning during early childhood. There is still little knowledge of long-term disadvantages associated with late-preterm birth. A majority of infants born late-preterm are also born with intrauterine growth restriction, which has been shown to increase risk of adverse outcomes in executive functioning in childhood and also in early adulthood. Although these factors have similar adverse effects in executive functions during childhood, there is little research of their possible additive effects. Previous studies have also used mainly performance-based methods to measure executive functions, which places doubts on ecological validity of previous results. The aim of this study is to examine whether late-preterm birth, intrauterine growth restriction or their additive effects are connected to self-rated executive functioning in early adulthood. Methods. The study sample comprised 2193 regionally sampled infants, who were born 1985-1986 and participated in the Arvo Ylppö Longitudinal Study (AYLS). Basic measures from peri- and neonatal periods were extracted from maternity hospital records and maternal interviews. Intrauterine growth restriction was estimated with being born small for gestational age (SGA). The participants' gestational age was reliably specified and they filled in the standardized and self-report based Behavioral Rating Inventory of Executive Functioning- Adult Version (BRIEF-A) at age of 24-26 years. The final sample comprised 634 participants, of whom 88 were born late-preterm and 37 were born SGA. The effects of late-preterm and SGA birth to self-reported executive functioning in early adulthood were analyzed using multiple-hierarchical-linear-regression, controlling for multiple confounders. The additive effects were analyzed with multiple MANOVA. Results and conclusions. Late-preterm birth and additive effects of late-preterm birth and SGA were not associated with adverse effects in self-rated executive functioning in adulthood, although negative trend was found. SGA was significantly associated with fewer adversities in self rated executive functioning. The systematic loss found in the study sample and the possibly positively skewed views of SGA adults may have affected found connections. Therefore, more research is needed from effects of late-preterm birth and SGA on executive functioning in adulthood.
  • Havusalmi, Miia (2014)
    Introduction. Preterm birth (< 37 weeks of gestation) has been associated with increased health risks in infants, with increasing morbidity for shorter gestation times. However, recently more attention has been brought to the fact that even late preterm birth (34 – 36 weeks of gestation) significantly increases the risk of morbidity and need for hospitalization. Risks are elevated in infants who are also born small for gestational age. Late preterm birth has been associated with neuromotor and cognitive developmental delays and psychiatric symptoms in childhood. Adults born late preterm have been found to have reached lower levels of educational attainment and lower income. Some studies have associated late preterm birth with diagnoses of psychiatric disorders in adulthood. This study examines whether late preterm birth is also associated with self-assessed psychiatric symptoms in adulthood. The study also examines whether the participant's gender or birth weight relative to length of gestation is a moderating factor for this association. Methods. The participants of this study are a part of the Arvo Ylppö Longitudinal Study follow-up cohort, which consists of children born in 1985-86 in maternal hospitals in the district of Uusimaa, Finland. The psychiatric symptoms of the participants were measured at approximately the age of 25 years. Measurement of symptoms was conducted with the Adult Self Report –questionnaire. The association between late preterm birth and psychiatric symptoms was examined with linear regression models. Results and discussion. Late preterm birth was not associated with increased psychiatric symptoms in adulthood. The gender or relative birth weight of participants was not a moderating factor for this association. The results highlight the need to determine the specific risk factors that have contributed to the previously discovered associations between late preterm birth and adverse outcomes. This could help further determine the groups of individuals in risk for psychiatric symptoms.
  • Liesto, Sanna (2014)
    Objectives. The aim of the current study was to examine whether patients with mild traumatic brain injury (MTBI) have neurocognitive deficits in acute phase and in three month follow up after injury that can be detected using a brief neuropsychological battery. Second aim of the study was also to examine if there was a subgroup of patients with more neuropsychological deficits. The aim was also to compare self-reported symptoms and objective neuropsychological deficits. MTBI is known to cause neurocognitive deficits including dysfunctions in memory, attention and executive functions in some patients. Usually neurocognitive dysfunctions resolve in three months but a minority of MTBI patients are reported to continue experience dysfunctions longer than three months. Methods. 26 patients with MTBI and 23 healthy controls participated in this study. Patients were examined with neuropsychological tests three days after the trauma. Neuropsychological tests assessed the domains of memory, attention and executive functions. Second examination was conducted three months after the first one for both groups. At the first examination patients completed SCAT2 questionnaire concerning self-reported symptoms. Results and conclusions. On a group level the neuropsychological test performance of MTBI patients did not differ from healthy controls. A minority of patients with MTBI showed significant neurocognitive deficits. Some self-reported symptoms correlated to neuropsychological performance. In the future more research is needed to explore the factors contributing to long lasting deficits after a MTBI. Also new neuropsychological tests are needed to examine the subtle deficits of MTBI.
  • Honkanen, Heidi (2016)
    Objectives. Obesity and depression are global health problems that cause enormous burden to the society as well as to the individuals. There is supposed to be a bidirectional association between obesity and depression which means that obesity cause depression and depression cause obesity. Previous research has partially supported this view, although there have been only few studies focusing on bidirectional association so far. This study examined whether there is a bidirectional association between obesity and depressive symptoms and how potential confounders and risk factors explain this relationship. Methods. The sample of this study (n = 1272) is part of an ongoing Young Finns Study, which began in 1980. Young Finns Study is an extensive Finnish population based follow-up study that examines cardiovascular risk factors from childhood to adulthood. Subjects were randomly chosen from the national register and data originally consisted of 3596 children and youths. Weight and depressive symptoms were measured in 2001 and 2007 and the confounders and risk factors in 2001. Examined confounders and risk factors in this study were socioeconomic status, social support and health behavior. Weight was examined by calculating body mass index and depressive symptoms were measured by using a modified version of the Beck Depression Inventory. Subjects of this study were 30–45 years old in 2007. Results and conclusions. Bidirectional association between obesity and depressive symptoms was found in this study. However, obesity did not predict change in depression nor did depression predict change in obesity in six years. In addition, male gender, higher age, low socioeconomic status, higher consumption of alcohol and low attention to health behavior functioned as risk factors for obesity. Risk factors for depressive symptoms were female gender, lack of social support and low attention to health behavior. These factors also partly explained the association between obesity and depressive symptoms. To conclude, in the current study there was a bidirectional association between obesity and depression, which was partly explained by confounders and other risk factors.