Skip to main content
Login | Suomeksi | På svenska | In English

Browsing by study line "ei opintosuuntaa"

Sort by: Order: Results:

  • Palola, Jemina (2021)
    Aims of the study. Aphasia can be studied from the aspect of functional communication, in which case the focus is on how a person with aphasia is able to use language in their everyday environment. One way to study the functional communication of people with aphasia is to analyse discourse, as word-finding difficulties, which are common in aphasia, can affect discourse. Discourse can also be studied to determine whether the effects of an intervention are generalized to the level of functional communication. The aim of this thesis is to study the discourse of people with chronic aphasia, elicited using interview questions, in different phases of an intervention consisting of rTMS treatment (repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation) and Intensive Language Action Therapy (ILAT). The discourse features which are analysed include lexical diversity (verbs and nouns) and informativeness (correct information units). The results of the discourse analysis are compared to the results of linguistic tests (Boston Naming Test for nouns and Action Naming Test for verbs). Methods. The data consisted of the discourse of three participants. The discourse was elicited using interview questions. All the participants had chronic fluent aphasia. They participated in the University of Helsinki research project “Treatment-Induced Speech and Language Improvement and Neuroplasticity after Stroke”, during which they received both rTMS treatment and ILAT therapy. They were examined four times during the intervention: at baseline, after rTMS treatment period, after combined rTMS and ILAT period, and at follow-up (3 months after the intervention). The discourse samples were analysed using discourse analysis. The chosen measurements were measures of lexical diversity, i. e. verbs and nouns, and measures of information units. The results were analysed visually. In addition, group effect sizes were calculated. Regression lines were used to compare the results of the discourse analysis to the results of linguistic tests. Results and discussion. No clear changes were detected in lexical diversity and informativeness of the discourse of people with chronic aphasia during rTMS and ILAT. Thus, the intervention did not have effect on functional communication at group level. However, there was much variation between the participants as well as between the results of different measures. The efficiency of informativeness increased for all the participants and it also had the largest group effect size. There was a stronger correlation between the use of nouns in discourse and the results of Boston Naming Test than between the use of verbs in discourse and the results of Action Naming Test
  • Vuokko, Leena (2020)
    Objectives. Interprofessional collaboration has become increasingly important in health and social care. Collaboration with professionals in other fields is also part of the work of many speech therapists and the method has been found to benefit the work in many ways. The number of studies on interprofessional collaboration is also increasing, although the perspective of speech therapists is rarely considered. The purpose of this study was to investigate the experiences speech and language therapists and their team members of other professions have on interprofessional collaboration. The participants were primary health care employees working in two municipalities of the Helsinki Metropolitan Area. The interviewees were also asked about their views on the benefits and challenges of cooperation. In addition, it was investigated what skills the study period provided for the interviewees’ interprofessional work, as well as what qualities close cooperation requires from the employee. Methods. The study was conducted in February and March 2020 in two separate group interviews involving a total of six speech therapists, three psychologists, three occupational therapists and one nurse. The interviews were videotaped and transcribed into text files. The data was analysed by means of inductive content analysis, and the results were reviewed in the light of previous research and literature. Results and conclusions. The work of the speech therapists interviewed involved a variety of multidisciplinary practices, and the speech therapists collaborated with professionals in many other fields. The closest co-operation was in the employees' own offices, where professionals from different fields, among other things, consulted each other and met customers together. Interprofessional collaboration was thought to benefit the work in many ways, while the disadvantages were perceived to be minor. Interviewees felt that little attention was paid to interprofessional cooperation during their studies. Many felt that it would have been useful to get to know the job descriptions of future partners in particular during their studies. Based on the results of this study, speech therapists in primary health care seem to appreciate the opportunity for interprofessional collaboration. Cooperation seems to benefit their work as well as reduce work-related stress. For example, challenges in schedules, information flow, and workplace space solutions seem to make it difficult for workers to collaborate. Studies in speech therapy appear to provide rather limited skills for multidisciplinary work on the basis of this study. As cooperation with workers in other fields is usually part of the job description of speech therapists, the inclusion of multidisciplinarity more closely as part of speech therapy studies would be justified.
  • Turkkila, Roosa (2023)
    The purpose of this study was to examine how inverting the face upside down impacts the perception of audiovisual speech and especially the integration of sensory information during such speech perception. It is well known that face perception involves specialized mechanisms, and it has been suggested that inverting the face would impair the use of to these mechanisms. On the other hand, speech perception is also known to be a specialized process, categorized by a strong audiovisual summation. The aim of this study was to find out whether the strong integration of sensory information in the perception of audiovisual speech is one such specialized mechanism that impairs when the face is inverted. The potential difference in audiovisual summation between upright and inverted faces was investigated using subthreshold summation paradigm. In this paradigm, single and multisensory discrimination thresholds were measured by presenting subjects video clips of spoken syllables [pa] and [ka] masked with audiovisual noise. The task was to identify which of the syllables were presented. Then, by fitting the Minkowski metric to the separation thresholds it is possible to derive different summation models that describe the strength of summation. The study’s results suggest that visual speech perception was poorer when the faces were perceived upside down. However, the audiovisual summation was equally strong for both upright and inverted faces. These results align with the previous knowledge that visual speech perception is considerably impaired when the face is inverted. While there is limited previous research on the effect of inversion on audiovisual summation using a similar setup, this study indicates that auditory and visual speech are combined in the same manner for both upright and inverted faces. Therefore, it seems, that the mechanisms responsible for the strong summation taken place in audiovisual speech perception appear to be distinct from those processes that are impaired in face inversion.
  • Lanki, Emma (2023)
    Background and objectives. Hearing impairment can manifest as weak skills in several areas of speech and language. In previous studies, musical activities have been found to have a positive effect on the language skills of children with normal hearing (NH). The purpose of this thesis was to find out whether there is a difference between hearing-impaired (HI) and NH- children in picture naming skills or rapid serial naming, whether there is a connection between performance in different naming tests, and whether the amount of singing in the group was associated with naming performance. The thesis is part of the MULAPAPU study, which examines the effects of music intervention and singing on the speech perception and speech and language development of HI-children aged 0–7. Methods. The participants (N=37) were 1;11–6;10 aged children, of whom the HI-children (n=17) formed the research group and the NH-children (n=20) the control group. HI-children used bilateral cochlear implant (n=9), bilateral hearing aid (n=7) or combination of cochlear implant and hearing aid (n=1). Based on the amount of singing in the group, the children were divided into a group of children that sang more than once a week (n=19) and a group that sang less than once a week (n=14). Both groups included HI-children and NH-children. Naming skills were assessed with the Bo Ege test and the Rapid Automatized Naming test (colors and objects subtests). The amount of singing in the group was measured using a questionnaire that was sent to the children’s parents. The data was analyzed using statistical methods. Results and conclusions. No statistically significant difference was found between the performance of HI-children and NH-children in the Bo Ege test. In the Rapid Automatized Naming test, the NH-children performed significantly faster in the objects subtest. Regarding the errors made, no differences were found between the groups. No correlation was found between the performance in the Bo Ege test and the Rapid Automatized Naming test, which suggest that naming of individual pictures and serial naming should be evaluated separately in clinical work. Singing in the group was not related to the performance in the Bo Ege test. In the Rapid Automatized Naming test, children who sing more than once a week performed both subtests faster on average and made fewer errors in the colors subtest, while children who sing less made fewer errors on average in the objects subtest. The differences were not statistically significant. However, the results suggest, together with previous studies, that singing in a group could be beneficial for the development of children's rapid naming skills.
  • Huovinen, Anni (2021)
    Aims. In previous studies a link between nonword repetition skills and children’s language skills such as phonological knowledge and reading skills. Musical activity has been found to improve children’s cognitive development and language skills. Nonword repetition and nonword span have been widely used as a measure of phonological skills and working memory. The aim of the study was to find out whether there is a connection between singing at home and phonological skills or working memory in children with hearing impairment. The second aim of the study was to find out whether there is a correlation of performance in these two tasks and is there a decrease in parental singing as the child grows older. Methods. The study was conducted as a multiple case study. The participants were recruited from CIsumusa, a speech-music playschool for hearing-impaired children. The participants were nine Finnish-speaking hearing-impaired children aged 1–6 years who used either cochlear implants or hearing aids or both. The data was comprised of nonword repetition and nonword span tasks completed by the children. The parents of the participants filled some questionnaires about musical activity of the family which were also analyzed in the study. The children were divided into four groups for further paired analysis: 1) the children whose parents sing to them daily, 2) those whose parents sing to them less frequently than daily, 3) the children who sing daily, 4) the children who sing less frequently than daily. Nonparametric methods (such as Mann Whitney U-test) were used for statistical analysis. Results and Conclusions. No statistically significant differences were found between singing groups. A statistically significant correlation was found between performances in nonword repetition and nonword span tasks. It was found that the children over 4 years and 3 months old repeated the nonwords significantly more correctly by the number of syllables than the younger participants. It was also found that the children whose parents sing to them daily were on average 13,45 months younger than the children whose parents sing to them less frequently, even though the age difference was not statistically significant. Due to small number of subjects the results may be considered indicative, but based on them and previous research, singing should be used in speech therapy rehabilitation and home training.
  • Savelius, Viivi (2023)
    Objectives. Hearing impairments (HIs) can cause delays in language skills. HIs can for example affect naming skills or the ability to perceive and produce word stress. Musical activity and singing have been shown beneficial to language skills of children with HIs. There is a lack of studies about connections between music and naming skills especially in children with HIs. The aim of this study was to examine how music intervention is linked to the production and perception of word stress in children with bilateral cochlear implants (CI) and/or hearing aids (HA). This study also examined is singing at home linked to better performance in rapid automatized naming test (RAN) or to the production and perception of word stress in nonword repetition task. This pilot study is a part of MULAPAPU research project which aims to study the effects of musical activity and singing to language skills in children with HI aged from 0 to 7 years. Methods. The participating children (n=19) with bilateral CIs and/or HAs were aged from 23 to 77 months. They were grouped based on answers given by their parents regarding the amount of singing at home (frequent singers and non-frequent singers). Production of word stress was assessed by using a non-word repetition-task invented in MULAPAPU research project and naming skills were evaluated by using the Rapid Automatized Naming -test (RAN). This study was a cross-over study in which group AB started with the intervention and group BA started with wash-up period. Both groups were tested three times. Group AB was tested before the intervention (T1), right after the intervention (T2) and after the wash-up period (T3). Group BA was tested before wash-up period (T1), right before intervention (T2) and right after the intervention (T3). Results and conclusions. Singing at home was linked to the time used and self-corrections in the RAN-test but there was no link between singing at home and the non-word repetition-task. AB groups word-stress production improved during the wash-up period. The effects of the music intervention might start to show with a delay. There were links between the children’s performances in the non-word repetition-task and RAN-test. This indicates that there might be a connection between word-stress production and naming. The results of this study are mainly in line with previous studies, and they support the evidence that music is a valuable asset in the rehabilitation of children with hearing impairment.
  • Kurittu, Pinja (2023)
    Hearing impairment is a risk factor for speech and language development. Previous studies with hearing-impaired children have shown that their vocabulary development is slower than normal, and they have challenges in the use of inflectional forms. The purpose of this master's thesis is to find out if there is a difference in the proficiency of expressive vocabulary or inflections and sentences in Finnish-speaking, bilaterally hearing-impaired children compared to children with normal hearing. In addition, the effect of singing on the observed language skills is examined. The thesis is part of the MULAPAPU research project, which investigates the effects of music on the language development of hearing-impaired children aged 0-7. The study included both hearing-impaired subjects (n=23) and controls with normal hearing (n=25). The subjects were between 1 and 6 years old. Hearing-impaired subjects were bilaterally using a cochlear implant, a hearing aid, or a combination of these. The exclusion criteria for both the group of hearing impaired and controls were any severe diagnosed comorbid disorders. Expressive vocabulary proficiency was assessed with the Leinikki questionnaire completed by parents, and proficiency in the use of inflectional forms and sentences was assessed with the MCDI questionnaire. Singing was surveyed with the MULAPAPU background survey. Differences in expressive vocabulary proficiency were statistically analyzed using the Mann-Whitney U-test, and the connections between parental singing, child’s singing and singing in a group, and expressive vocabulary proficiency were analyzed using correlation analysis. Very few control subjects had answered the MCDI questionnaire's section of inflectional forms and sentences, so the skills of hearing-impaired children were analyzed on an individual level by comparing their scores to the standard values of the questionnaire. No statistically significant difference in expressive vocabulary proficiency was observed between hearing-impaired and normally hearing children. However, the hearing-impaired group had a two-peaked distribution and some subjects had poor expressive vocabulary proficiency. Hearing-impaired children's proficiency in inflections and sentences fell short of the age norms, although proportionate to hearing age, some of the subjects had a good proficiency in inflections and sentences. Only the child singing was statistically significantly correlated to expressive vocabulary proficiency. The amount of singing by parents was also statistically significantly related to the amount of singing by the child. Guiding the close environment and encouraging children to sing can be a good part of rehabilitating the language skills of hearing-impaired children.
  • Eerikäinen, Tiiti (2021)
    Objectives. Hearing impaired (HI) children have been found to perform more poorly in tasks measuring semantic verbal fluency compared to normal hearing peers. Music activities are known to have a positive connection to the word finding skills in HI-children. However, there has not been yet studies to find out whether organized music activities have an effect on semantic verbal fluency skills in young HI children. The purpose of this study was to determine how the number of accepted words in semantic verbal fluency task develops during a music intervention in HI-children and whether background variables (age, mother’s education and hearing device) affect development. There was also a desire to find out the effect of singing at home on the development of verbal fluency task. In addition, it was examined whether the word retrieval strategies change during the music intervention and whether they are influenced by background variables. Methods. The participants (n = 15) were HI-children aged 2–6 years. Participants were divided into non-singers and singers based how much their parents sang at home. Verbal fluency (VF) and verbal fluency strategies were measured with two semantic VF tasks (animals and clothes). Tests were performed before (T1) and after (T2) the children participated in CIsumusa music intervention, organized by LapCi ry. The results were analyzed by statistical methods. Results and conclusions. The participants developed in verbal fluency task during the music intervention in both animal and clothing categories. Higher age was strongly associated with better development in both categories. Mother’s education was related to development in the animal task. Children of non-singers developed during the music intervention, but children of singers did not. However, children of singers performed better at T1 compared to children of non-singers. In a group of all participants the number of clusters increased during the music intervention in both animal and clothing tasks and the cluster size increased in the clothing task. Higher age was associated with an increase in the number of clusters, but not in cluster size. Based on the results, the music intervention was able to improve VF of children who performed poorly before the intervention. The results suggest that if there is no singing or musical activity at home, out-of-home music activities that include singing can improve VF. Singing at home and music interventions can be recommended to children with HI.
  • Parkkinen, Kaisla (2019)
    Aims. Past research has shown that hearing impaired adolescents are at a higher risk of experiencing psychosocial difficulties than their hearing peers. The research into the factors related to the psychosocial well-being of hearing-impaired adolescents has not yet come to an unambiguous conclusion. There are indications that the wellbeing of hard-of-hearing youths would be related to their communicative abilities and the functioning of their hearing among other things. This study examines self-perceived psychosocial difficulties and strengths of adolescents with hearing impairment and factors associated with these difficulties and strengths. These results also are being compared to results of psychosocial well-being obtained from Finnish adolescents. Methods. 12 11-17-year-old youths with hearing impairment filled in the Finnish version of the Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire (SDQ-Fin) and a background questionnaire. Adolescents with other sensory deficits or disabilities were excluded from this study. Statistical analysis was performed to search for associations between psychosocial well-being and its related factors. Results and conclusions. The participants experienced more psychosocial difficulties than their Finnish peers. Emotional symptoms and peer problems were emphasized in the reports of participants. They also experienced more strengths in psychosocial well-being compared to their peers. In this study girls experienced more difficulties than boys. Participants experienced fewer difficulties in psychosocial wellbeing when their communicative abilities were good. These results can be used to improve and develop rehabilitation of hearing-impaired children and youths.
  • Kuparinen, Heini (2022)
    Aim of the study: Risk-sensitive foraging theory (RSFT) is an evolutionary-biological theory of how animals should choose in uncertain foraging situations for the choice to be beneficial for survival and reproduction. When having low energy budget, a more risky choice would be beneficial, and when having high energy budget, one should avoid a risky decision. There is no one, exhaustive model for risk-sensitive behavior, RSFT being just one of them. It has earlier been established that dependence disorders have common factors causing harm, non-specific to drug, and alcohol use is associated with gambling, both through shared genetic and environmental factors, and directly, and this association might be mediated by risk taking. Also, differences in risk taking behavior are rather individual based than species related. It has even been found that heroin-dependent individuals favor more risky choices over certain ones when in need of the drug. The aim of this study was to find out whether RSFT is suitable for describing the risk taking related to gambling. Methods: The sample was from Gambling Impact and Behavior Study survey data, which was collected by random-digit dial method in USA in 1997–1999. Economical aspect of energy budget was operationalized as household income, mental aspect of energy budget as gambling problem (measured by NODS, scale 0–10), and the decision to risk as money spent on gambling. Respondents with missing values in any of the variables were excluded resulting in a final sample size of 893 individuals. It was examined if household income and gambling problem were associated with money spent on gambling. A general linear model was fitted to the sample. Results: A general liner model seemed to fit to the sample. The income variable didn’t improve the model, but gambling problem did. Moreover, income and gambling problem had a significant interaction. Only with a few exceptions, people played more money in higher income classes and with a more severe gambling problem. People played a little money in every income class even with no gambling problem at all. On the other hand, when the gambling problem was severe, it affected the amount spent on gambling more in high income classes than in lower ones. People in the lowest income classes spent nearly the same amount with severe than with no gambling problem. Conclusions: The results give mixed implications of whether RSFT describes the risk taking related to gambling, which might be due to the unprecise definition of variables and methods of analysis, and other methodological aspects specific to gambling. The twin threshold model holds more promise than the traditional model of RSFT: when both economical and mental energy budgets are low, risk is avoided in gambling (survival threshold), but when economical energy budget is high risk is favored, which however, comes short as the definition of reproductive threshold in the context of gambling. A further examination is advised before making firm conclusions. Most importantly, gambling and gambling problems might be better understood from other perspectives.
  • Ihalin, Meeri (2020)
    Background and the aims of the study. Previous studies show that adult immigrants face chal-lenges especially in producing diphthongs and differences in Finnish speech sound lengths. However, to date there has been no evaluation method to assess immigrant’s pronunciation in Finnish. The purpose of this study was to determine whether the Phonology test is applicable for assessing adult immigrant’s pronunciation of Finnish. This applicability was assessed by examining what the results of the Phonology test revealed about research subject’s pronun-ciation, how these results differed from the ones of a sentence test, which was developed for this study in particular, how well they recognised the words in the Phonology test. In addition, how and for whom the test could be applicable in future were also studied. Research subjects and methods. The study was carried out as a comperative multi-case stu-dy in which the data was examined both quantitatively and qualitatively. The data was obtai-ned as a part of a PhD dissertation at The University of the Arts Helsinki examining the effects of choir singing in adult immigrant’s Finnish language learning. The research data consisted of ten adult immigrant’s recordings and research forms of the Phonology test and sentence test. The data were statistically analysed with SPSS-program using a dependent sample t-test and the Pearson correlation coefficient. Results and conclusions. All research subjects received rather good scores in the Phonology test and there was not any significant deviation. The results of the Phonology test and the sentence test were quite similar in terms of the mean difference in speech sound length. The pronounciation challenges appeared in the Phonology test were also found in the sentence test and vice versa. The subjects were able to produce short speech sounds distincly shorter than long speech sounds in almost every word in the sentence test. Principally, the subjects recognised the words in the Phonology test well. Based on these results, the Phonology test was moderately applicable to assess adult immigrants’ pronunciation of Finnish.
  • Taskinen, Anette (2022)
    Objectives: Socioeconomic status (SES) in childhood has been found to be in association with later mental disorders however, there has been less study into effects of durance and especially timing of socioeconomic status. Identifying risks related to timing of and cumulative SES can help target resources in preventative manner to those who need it the most. The objective of this study is to examine the relationship between timing and cumulative SES in childhood and later mental health problems. Methods: Data of this study is part of Millenium Cohort Study collected in United Kingdom. Sample in this study was N=7226. Parents income when the cohort members were 9 months, 3, 5, and 7 years old was used as an indicator of socioeconomic position and later problems with mental health were assessed with Kesslers K6 and Strengths and Difficulties Questionnaire when the participants were 17 years old. Association between childhood socioeconomic position and later mental health problems was examined with logistic regression analysis. Results: Socioeconomic status during childhood in general was associated with mental health problems at the age of 17. The odds ratios had a downward trend; as the income level rose in both analysis of incomes separately at different ages as well as when looking at the cumulative income. In the former, especially in the lowest and second lowest income quantile there was a higher risk for later mental health problems compared to the highest income quantile. There was no difference in risks associated with income quantiles between different age stages, however some trends were observed. Cumulative socioeconomic status was mainly more strongly associated with risk of later mental health problems compared to risks when examining income at single stage. Conclusions. In this longitudinal study SES during childhood did not explain the magnitude of risk for mental health problems at the age of 17, but rather being a part of the lowest income quantiles was relevant despite the timing. In comparison the durance of SES was a better predictor of problems with mental health compared to SES at separate age stages. The risks associated with income levels were mostly bigger in earlier ages, but the confidence intervals for the odds ratios were large and the inter-rater reliability between the outcome measures was poor. In future studies these observations should be noted in the design of the study.
  • Reijonen, Karoliina (2019)
    Tavoitteet. Tässä tutkimuksessa oli tavoitteena tutkia, miten 5- ja 9- vuotiaiden lasten lyhytkestoisen muistin toimintaa mittaavat testien tulokset ovat yhteydessä aikuisena 40- vuotiaiden työmuistisuoriutumiseen sekä visuaaliseen ja verbaaliseen päättelykykyyn. Tutkimuskysymykset olivat: 1) Miten lapsuuden auditiivisen ja visuaalisen sarjamuistin tehtävissä suoriutuminen on yhteydessä aikuisuuden auditiivisverbaaliseen työmuistisuoriutumiseen? 2) Miten lapsuuden auditiivisen ja visuaalisen sarjamuistin tehtävissä suoriutuminen on yhteydessä työmuistisuoriutumiseen, jossa informaatiota manipuloidaan? 3) Miten lapsuuden auditiivisen ja visuaalisen sarjamuistin tehtävissä suoriutuminen on yhteydessä aikuisuuden kielelliseen ja visuaaliseen päättelykykyyn? Menetelmät. Tutkimusaineistona oli Kognition pitkäaikaistutkimus (KOPUTUS), jossa oli vuosina 1971- 1974 syntyneitä syntymäriskiryhmiin kuuluvia lapsia sekä kontrolliryhmä. Tähän tutkimukseen otettiin mukaan 5-, 9-, ja 40- vuotiaiden tutkimustulokset (N=301). Lapsuudessa tutkittavat suorittivat lasten kommunikaatiota ja kielellisiä kykyjä mittaavan The Illinois Test of Psycholinguistic Abilities (ITPA)- testin. Muuttujiksi valittiin lyhytkestoista muistia mittaavat testit: visuaalinen sarjamuisti ja auditiivinen sarjamuisti. Aikuisuudessa tutkittavat suorittivat WAIS- IV:n, josta muuttujiksi valittiin työmuistia mittaava perusosatehtävä Numerosarjat sekä kielellisen ja visuaalisen päättelyn indeksit (VCI ja PRI). Taustamuuttujiksi tutkimukseen otettiin tutkittavien oma korkein koulutus, äidin korkein koulutus ja turvapisteet 9- vuotiaana. Tutkimustulokset analysoitiin rakenneyhtälömallien avulla. Tulokset ja johtopäätökset. Tulokset osoittivat, että 5- vuoden ikäisten ja 9- vuoden ikäisten auditiivisen lyhytkestoisen muistin suoriutumiset erikseen ennustivat aikuisuuden työmuistisuoriutumista. Lisäksi 9- vuoden ikäisten auditiivisella lyhytkestoisella muistilla voitiin ennustaa myös monimutkaisempaa työmuistin toimintaa. Visuaalisella lyhytkestoisella muistilla lapsuudessa taas voitiin ennustaa visuaalista päättelykykyä aikuisena. Tuloksista voidaan päätellä, että lasten lyhytkestoisen muistin tutkiminen on tärkeää, koska sillä voidaan ennustaa aikuisuuden työmuistin toimintaa ja visuaalisen tiedon käsittelyä sekä mahdollisia heikentymiä näissä varhain.
  • Smedman, Nadja (2022)
    The continuum in child language development is a well-known phenomenon. Language skills consist of several different language domains that a child must learn/accomplish to become a fluent language user. Language acquisition is a complex process. It is still partly unknown how the different language domains develop in relation to each other and how they associate to later language skills. The purpose of this Master’s thesis is to find out the longitudinal associations between and predictive value of the mastery of lexicon and language structures of a 3 to 4 year old child and language skills at the age of five. The study aims to look at the connections between a child’s expressive and receptive lexicon and language structures (morphology, syntax and phonology) and later language skills. The sample of this study consisted of 92 children who participated in the LEINIKKI research project. Participants were healthy and typically developing children who acquired Finnish as their first language. The language skills at the age of 3 to 4 were assessed using the LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assess vocabulary, morphology, syntax and phonology and WPPSI-III subscales assessing expressive and receptive lexical skills. At the age of five language skills were assessed again using The Children’s Communication Checklist (CCC-2). The relationships between age 3- to 4-year-old and 5-year-old language proficiency were examined using correlation coefficients and regression models. The results of this study showed that there were several statistically significant and moderately strong correlations between the expressive lexical skills of a 3- to 4-year-old child and the language skills at five measured by both the LEINIKKI method and the WPPSI-III method. The association between toddlers’ receptive lexical skills and language skills at five was not significant. Several statistically significant and moderately strong associations were also found between the mastery of language structures of a 3- to 4-year-old child and the language proficiency at the age of five. The LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assessed morphology and syntax correlated with almost all aspects of language proficiency at the age of five and with the variable measuring overall language proficiency. The LEINIKKI method subscale, which measures speech clarity and sound system, correlated with five-year-old speech clarity and overall language proficiency. Both the lexicon and language structures of the 3- to 4-year-old child significantly explained language proficiency at the age of five years. The best models for explaining later language proficiency included variables of both 3-4-year-old lexicon and language structure variables and explained about 30 % of the language proficiency at the age of five years. Based on the results of this study, attention should be paid to the mastery of both lexicon and grammar in the assessment of a 3- to 4-year-old child's language skills.
  • Kaila, Elisabet (2020)
    Semantic fluency task is often used as part of an assesment to investigate children’s lexical development. Semantic fluency task measures the ability to generate words within a certain category and within a restricted amount of time. In previous studies, children’s semantic fluency skills have increased as the child grows. In addition, word retrieval strategies have been found to enhance word recall in the semantic fluency task with school-aged children and adults. However, there is only little knowledge regarding the use of word retrieval strategies in the semantic fluency task with preschool aged children. So far, in previous studies word retrieval strategies in the task have not been investigated with children under the age of 4. The aim of this study was to examine whether there are differences in 2 to 5 year old children’s performance in the semantic fluency task and whether children’s gender or their parent’s socioeconomic status had an impact on their performance. In addition, the children’s ability of using word retrieval strategies were explored and the relation between the use of word retrieval strategies and the number of correct words were evaluated. The study sample included 79 children. Children completed two semantic fluency tasks guided by their parents in an online questionnaire. During the tasks, the children generated words belonging to the semantic category of animals and clothes within a minute. The performance was evaluated on the basis of the number of correct words, errors and error types. Word retrieval strategies were studied by examining clusters, semantic subcategories and switches between subcategories. The results of this study showed that age had a strong relation to performance on fluency tasks as measured with a number of correct words, clusters, semantic subcategories and switches. The new result was that even 2 year old children could generate correct words for the task and utilize word retrieval strategies in the tasks. The children’s gender or parents’ socioeconomic status had no effect on performance in the semantic fluency tasks. Number of clusters, semantic subcategories and switches had a positive relation to the number of correct words which indicated that successful word retrieval required use of fluency strategies even with preschool aged children. However, due to the lack of previous research data, more research regarding children’s word retrieval strategies is necessary. This study is the first in which the children’s word fluency task was guided by their parents. Clinical practice and further studies could benefit from these results that such a research design would appear to be suitable for assessing children’s semantic fluency.
  • Heimala, Silja (2024)
    Tavoitteet. Laulamisella on positiivisia vaikutuksia mielenterveyteen ja hyvinvointiin sekä kliinisessä kontekstissa että arkisesti käytettynä. Aiempi tutkimusnäyttö kuorolaulun yhteyksistä parempaan elämänlaatuun ja alhaisempaan masentuneisuuteen perustuu kuitenkin pitkälti interventiotutkimuksiin rajatulla kohderyhmällä. Lisäksi muut lauluharrastusten muodot, kuten yksin- ja yhteislaulu, ovat jääneet tutkimuksessa taka-alalle. Vapaamuotoisemman lauluharrastamisen yhteyksistä hyvinvointiin eri vaiheissa aikuisikää tarvitaan systemaattista tutkimusnäyttöä. Tämän tutkielman tavoitteena on selvittää poikkileikkausasetelmaa hyödyntäen kuoro-, yksin- ja yhteislaulun yhteyksiä elämänlaatuun, sosiaaliseen hyvinvointiin ja masentuneisuuteen eri vaiheissa aikuisikää. Menetelmät. Tutkimukseen osallistui N = 100 iältään 21–88-vuotiasta vapaaehtoista, joilla oli vaihtelevissa määrin kuoro-, yksin- tai yhteislaulukokemusta. Lauluharrastusten yhteyksiä elämänlaatuun, sosiaaliseen hyvinvointiin ja masentuneisuuteen selvitettiin lineaarisella regressiolla. Koko otoksessa tilastollisesti merkitsevät yhteydet analysoitiin myös ikäryhmittäin (nuoret, keski-ikäiset, ikääntyvät), jotta voitiin tarkastella elämänvaiheen ja ikääntymisen vaikutuksia tulosten voimakkuuteen. Musiikkitaustaa arvioitiin the Goldsmiths Musical Sophistication Index (Gold-MSI) -kyselyyn pohjautuvilla kysymyksillä. Elämänlaatua arvioitiin The World Health Organization Quality of Life (WHOQOL) -BREF-kyselyllä, sosiaalista tukea Social Provision Scale (SPS) -kyselyllä ja masentuneisuutta Center for Epiodemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D) -kyselyllä. Tulokset ja johtopäätökset. Kuorolaulun nykyinen viikoittainen aktiivisuustaso oli tilastollisesti merkitsevässä yhteydessä alhaisempaan CES-D-pistemäärään eli alhaisempaan masentuneisuuteen. Kyseinen tulos ei ollut tilastollisesti merkitsevä ikäryhmittäin analysoituna. Elämänlaadusta ja sosiaalisesta hyvinvoinnista ei tässä tutkielmassa saatu tilastollisesti merkitseviä tuloksia, mikä voi mahdollisesti selittyä lineaarisen regressioanalyysin oletusten rikkoutumisella. Tulevaisuudessa olisi keskeistä kartoittaa laulumuotojen vaikutuksia vertailevalla interventioasetelmalla tai laadullisella tutkimuksella.
  • Kuokkanen, Emma (2024)
    The aim of the study. The importance of emotional intelligence in the 21st century has grown considerably, when it comes to the world of work, and has created an emergence of research literature around the topic in the past decades. Especially, when talking about leadership, there is an increasing emphasis on emotional, communication and interpersonal skills that are needed for leaders to navigate in the ever-changing work environment and to tackle different, multidimensional challenges. The current world situation and changes in it has also placed new demands on both employees and managers as well as probably permanently altered the way we do work and have understood it previously. Today, leadership ability is often studied through competences, as they provide an easily understandable and measurable approach to a demanding and complex subject. The purpose of this study was to explore the extent to which emotional intelligence and its different scales are associated with leadership competences and differentiate the most relevant applications these findings have in real life. The impact of gender and job level was also examined. Methods. The data consisted of 482 individuals who took part in competence assessments, as part of recruitment processes, conducted by a private talent management consultancy between 2020 and 2023. Their emotional intelligence was assessed by the EIP3 questionnaire, which measures the overall emotional intelligence and additionally, the 16 subscales of emotional intelligence. Furthermore, the respondents also completed the PAPI3+ questionnaire, which was used to assess their ability in three different leadership competences: ’Coaching and developing’, ’Motivating and engaging’, and ’Resilience’. The relationship between emotional intelligence, gender and job level, and the three leadership competences was examined using regression models. Results. The results of the study showed a statistically significant association between emotional intelligence and leadership competence. When looking at each of the three leadership competences, statistically significant associations were found across a number of different emotional intelligence subscales, indicating that emotional intelligence is indeed an important key factor underlying leadership ability. Conclusions. The findings in this study highlights emotional intelligence as a crucial factor when predicting leadership performance and competence. Future research efforts should focus on understanding this connection in more depth and studying the effect of emotional intelligence on other leadership competences as well.
  • Rajala, Nea (2021)
    Objectives. Even though there have been more Finnish publications focusing on children’s narrative abilities, the knowledge around the topic is still minor. Also, the relationship between narrative abilities and other language development and later learning skills have not been studied in Finnish at all, while these skills have been associated to one another in international research. The aim of this study was to examine the narrative ability of a preschool-aged child. In addition to this, the relationship between narrative abilities and vocabulary was examined. Methods. This study used data from the LEINIKKI research project. The participants were 10 (five girls and five boys) healthy monolingual Finnish-speaking children aged between 3 years 7 months and 4 years. Children’s narrative abilities were assessed by the Cat Story. The stories were also analyzed using the story grammar rules by Stein and Glenn (1979). There were also other variables collected from the children’s stories; number of different words (type-variable), total number of words (token-variable) and the ratio between these two, type/token-ratio (TTR). Children’s vocabulary was assessed with vocabulary sections of Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence 3rd edition and LEINIKKI. The data was described and the results illustrated with descriptive statistics and visual descriptors. Spearman's rank correlation coefficient was used to examine the relationships between skills. Results and conclusions. The children named characters from the picture booklet and narrated events in them, but the stories were not accurate in conveying meaning of the content and the plots were not whole. Every child’s story did still have some story grammar elements in it. The productivity of these stories, evaluated by the type- and token-variable values, and the ratio between them (TTR), had medium to large differences between different children. The children’s ability to use story grammar was observed to be positively correlated with the receptive vocabulary and TTR was shown to be negatively correlated with expressive vocabulary. More research is needed around the development of Finnish children’s narrative abilities and the connection between this and other language development and later learning skills. Knowledge of typical development is important, as it is a prerequisite for recognizing anomalous features and treating them.
  • Anttila, Elina (2022)
    Children’s language skills can be evaluated by formal testing, parent report or assessing spontaneous speech in recorded language samples. This study focused on measures of lexical diversity determined from children’s spontaneous speech. These measures are one way to assess children’s lexical skills. However, there is only little research on the relation between the most recent lexical diversity measures and validated measures of lexical skills. The first aim of this study was to find out if there is a relation between the lexical diversity assessed from spontaneous speech of preschool children and lexical skills assessed by validated methods. The second aim was to examine if the measures of lexical diversity predict the lexical skills of a child assessed by validated measures and if so, which of the measures is the best predictor of a child’s lexical skills. The data of the study was collected in LEINIKKI research project. The participants (N = 68) were 2;6–4;0-year-old, healthy, Finnish-speaking children. The spontaneous speech of the participants was recorded in free play settings, the speech of the children was transcribed and token, type, type-token ratio (TTR) and moving-average type-token ratio (MATTR) values were determined from the first ten minutes of each language sample. These values were used as measures of the children’s lexical diversity. The following validated methods were used to assess the children’s lexical skills: the lexical skills assessing part of the parent report method LEINIKKI and Wechsler Preschool and Primary Scale of Intelligence 3rd edition subtests Picture Naming and Receptive Vocabulary. The relation between the lexical diversity measures and the validated measures of lexical skills was examined by Spearman’s correlation. It was also researched if the measures of lexical diversity had the ability to differentiate the children who scored the least in the validated lexical measures. Linear regression models were used in examining whether the measures of lexical diversity predicted the lexical skills assessed by validated methods when the age of the children was considered as a background variable. The token values neither correlated with the children’s lexical skills nor differentiated the least scoring quarter in the validated lexical measures. The type and TTR values correlated significantly (p < 0,05) with two out of three validated measures of lexical skills but were not statistically significant predictors of the children’s lexical skills. The type value did not differentiate the least scoring quarter in the validated lexical measures whereas the TTR value did in two out of three validated lexical measures. The relation of the MATTR value to a child’s lexical skills was the most significant: the MATTR value correlated highly significantly (p < 0,001) with all validated measures of lexical skills, differentiated the least scoring quarter in all three validated lexical measures and was a highly significant (p < 0,001) predictor of lexical skills in all regression models. This study supports the use of the MATTR value instead of the more traditional measures of lexical diversity when assessing the lexical diversity of preschool children. The MATTR value can be used especially in supplementing the view of a child’s lexical skills acquired by other measures. However, more research on the topic is needed.
  • Kaivola, Anna (2020)
    In Finland there does not exists any assessment tool based on parent ratings which can be used to assess 2;6–4;0-year-old children’s language ability. The LEINIKKI method is under development to fill this gap. Before a new assessment method can be applied research has to exist about its reliability and validity to support conclusions made by the assessment method. The aim of this study was to gain information about the LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assess morphology and syntax skills when assessing preschool aged children. Reliability and validity were examined by internal consistency and concurrent validity, respectively. This study used data from the LEINIKKI research project. The sample included 60 healthy, monolingual Finnish-speaking children aged between 2 years 7 months and 4 years 1 month. In addition to parent report instrument (LEINIKKI) children’s language skills were assessed by examiner-administered tests (the Finnish Morphology test, the Finnish version of the Reynell Developmental Language Scales III). Internal consistency of the LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assess morphology and syntax skills was measured by the coefficient alpha and by examining the distribution of the answers. Concurrent validity was assessed by comparing the results from the LEINIKKI method and the examiner-administered tests by using correlation coefficients. Internal consistency of the LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assess morphology and syntax skills was high, although some items were very easy. Statistically significant correlations were found between the LEINIKKI method and the examiner-administered tests. The connections were moderately strong and positive. Thus, preliminary evidence of concurrent validity was achieved. Results indicate that the LEINIKKI method’s “Inflected forms and grammatical structures” subscale functions better with children under 3 years 6 months and “Language complexity” subscale functions better with children older than 3 years 6 months. Using the Mean Length of Three Longest Utterances (M3L value) in this population seems promising if the instructions can be modified so that parents understand them more consistently. This thesis focuses only on LEINIKKI method’s subscales that assess morphology and syntax skills. Results about the whole assessment tool’s usability and validity will be gotten after the normative study of the LEINIKKI method is ready.